Kenneth Vercammen is a Middlesex County trial attorney who has published 130 articles in national and New Jersey publications on Criminal Law and litigation topics. Appointments can be scheduled at 732-572-0500. He is author of the ABA's book "Criminal Law Forms".
2053 Woodbridge Avenue - Edison, NJ 08817
http://www.njlaws.com/

Thursday, February 11, 2016

IDENTIFICATION report & lineups in NJ

IDENTIFICATION report & lineups in NJ
page1image1712
REPORT OF THE SUPREME COURT COMMITTEE ON MODEL CRIMINAL JURY CHARGES ON THE REVISIONS TO THE IDENTIFICATION MODEL CHARGES
Approved by Supreme Court Committee on Model Criminal Jury Charges January 9, 2012
page1image6760
TABLE OF CONTENTS
PAGE I. OVERVIEW ......................................................................................................... 1 II REVISION OF IDENTIFICATION MODEL JURY CHARGES ............................2
III. IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS CHARGE.....................3
  1. First Two Paragraphs ............................................................................... 3
    1. Committees Discussion ................................................................3
    2. Recommended Language .............................................................4
  2. Inclusion of References to Scientific Studies and Research.....................5
1. Committees Discussion ................................................................5 C. Inclusion of Detailed Description on Memory and its Stages .................... 7
  1. Committees Discussion ................................................................7
  2. Recommended Third Paragraph ...................................................9
  1. Fourth and Fifth Paragraphs on Evaluating the Identification...............10
    1. Committees Discussion ..............................................................10
    2. Recommended Language ...........................................................10
  2. Introductory Paragraph for the Estimator Factors...................................11 1. Committees Discussion ..............................................................11 2. Recommended Paragraph...........................................................12
  3. The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention Factor ....... 13 1. Committees Discussion .............................................................. 13 2. Recommended Paragraph...........................................................13
  4. Stress Factor .......................................................................................... 14
    1. Courts Findings ........................................................................... 14
    2. Committees Discussion .............................................................. 14
    3. Recommended Paragraph...........................................................15
  5. Weapons Focus Factor .......................................................................... 16
    1. Courts Findings ........................................................................... 16
    2. Committees Discussion .............................................................. 16
    3. Recommended Paragraph...........................................................17
  6. Duration Factor ....................................................................................... 18
    1. Courts Findings ........................................................................... 18
    2. Committees Discussion .............................................................. 18
    3. Recommended Paragraph...........................................................19
  7. Distance and Lighting Factors ................................................................ 20
    1. Courts Findings ........................................................................... 20
    2. Committees Discussion .............................................................. 20
    3. Recommended Paragraphs ......................................................... 21
page2image15968 page2image16128
i
PAGE
  1. Intoxication Factor ............................................................................... 22
    1. Courts Findings ........................................................................... 22
    2. Committees Discussion .............................................................. 22
    3. Recommended Paragraph...........................................................24
  2. Disguises/Changed Appearance Factor ................................................. 25
page3image2920
1. 2. 3.
M. Prior 1.
Courts Findings .................................................................... 25 Committees Discussion .............................................................. 25 Recommended Paragraph...........................................................25
Description of Perpetrator Factor ................................................... 26 Committees Discussion ..............................................................26 Recommended Paragraph...........................................................27
2.
  1. Confidence and Accuracy Factor
    1. Courts Findings
    2. Committees Discussion
    3. Recommended Paragraph
  2. Time Elapsed Factor
    1. Courts Findings
    2. Committees Discussion
    3. Recommended Paragraph
  3. Cross-Racial Effects Factor
    1. Courts Findings
    2. Committees Discussion
    3. Recommended Paragraph
........................................................... 28 .......................................................... 28 .......................................................... 28 .......................................................... 29 .......................................................... 30 .......................................................... 30 .......................................................... 30 .......................................................... 31 .......................................................... 32 .......................................................... 32 .......................................................... 32 .......................................................... 33 .......................................................... 34
Q. Catch-all Factor
  1. Original Factors on Child Witnesses and Own-Age Bias ........34
  2. Recommended Paragraph ..........................................................35
R. Inconsistent Statements Factor .......................................................... 36 1. Committees Discussion ..........................................................36
  1. Introductory Paragraph for the Systems Factors....................................37
    1. Committees Discussion ..........................................................37
    2. Recommended Paragraph ..........................................................37
  2. Lineup Composition and Fillers Factors ................................................. 38
    1. Courts Findings .......................................................... 38
    2. Committees Discussion .......................................................... 39
    3. Recommended Paragraphs ......................................................... 39
  1. Multiple Viewings Factor
    1. Courts Findings
    2. Committees Discussion
    3. Recommended Paragraph
  2. Showups Factor
    1. Courts Findings
    2. Committees Discussion
    3. Recommended Paragraph
.......................................................... 40 .......................................................... 40 .......................................................... 41 .......................................................... 41 .......................................................... 42 .......................................................... 42 .......................................................... 43 .......................................................... 46
ii
PAGE
  1. Identification Lineup Procedure .......................................................... 47
    1. Lineup Administrator Factor ......................................................... 47
      1. Courts Findings .......................................................... 47
      2. Committees Discussion .................................................... 48
      3. Recommended Paragraph ................................................ 48
    2. Instructions Factor .......................................................... 50
      1. Courts Findings .......................................................... 50
      2. Committees Discussion .................................................... 50
      3. Recommended Paragraph ................................................ 50
    3. Feedback Factor .......................................................... 52 a. Courts Findings .......................................................... 52 b. Committees Discussion....................................................52 c. Recommended Paragraph ................................................ 53
  2. Concluding Paragraphs .......................................................... 54
    1. Committees Discussion ..........................................................54
    2. Recommended Paragraphs.........................................................54
APPENDIX A
Proposed Charges
  1. Identification: In-Court and Out-of-Court Identifications
  2. Identification: In-Court Identification Only
  3. Identification: Out-of-Court Identification Only
APPENDIX B
Current Charges
  1. Identification: In-Court and Out-of-Court Identifications
  2. Identification: In-Court Identification Only
  3. Identification: Out-of-Court Identification Only
page4image10808 page4image10968 page4image11128
iii
I. OVERVIEW
In State v. Henderson, 208 N.J. 208, 298 (2011), the Court asked the Criminal Practice Committee and the Model Criminal Jury Charge Committee to draft proposed revisions to the current charges on eyewitness identification and submit them to the Court for approval.
Based upon the scientific evidence in the record the Court was convinced:
[M]emory is malleable, and that an array of variables can affect and dilute memory and lead to misidentifications. Those factors include system variables like lineup procedures, which are within the control of the criminal justice system, and estimator variables like lighting conditions or the presence of a weapon, over which the legal system has no control.
[ Id. at 218.]
Therefore, the Court concluded:

[T]he current standard for assessing eyewitness identification evidence does not fully meet its goals. It does not offer an adequate measure for reliability or sufficiently deter inappropriate police conduct. It also overstates the jurys inherent ability to evaluate evidence offered by eyewitnesses who honestly believe their testimony is accurate.
[ Ibid.]
In order to remedy its concerns regarding the jury charges, the Court

page5image11016 page5image11176 page5image11336 page5image11496
requested that:
[T]he court system ... develop enhanced jury charges on eyewitness identification for trial judges to use. We anticipate that identification evidence will continue to be admitted in the vast majority of cases. To help jurors weigh that evidence, they must be told about relevant factors and their effect on reliability...With the use of more focused jury charges on those issues, there will be less need to call expert witnesses at trial. Trial courts will still have discretion to admit expert testimony when warranted.
[ Id. at 218-19.]
page5image17080
1
II. REVISION OF IDENTIFICATION MODEL JURY CHARGES
Scientific literature divides the variables that can affect and dilute memory and lead to misidentification into two categories: System and Estimator variables. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 247. In calling for a revision of the Identification model jury charges, the Court charged the Committees to consider all of the system and estimator variables in section VI for which we have found scientific support that is generally accepted by experts.... Id. at 298-99. In response to this charge, the Model Criminal Jury Charge Committee formed a subcommittee to draft revisions of the Identification charges for the full Committees review.
The Committee reviewed and modified the subcommittees proposed revision, using the In-Court and Out-of-Court Identifications charge as a template. The charge was unanimously approved by the Committee. The Committee decided that the language approved in that charge could then be incorporated into the other two Identification charges: In-Court Identification Only and Out-ofCourt Identification Only. The Committees discussion regarding each section of the proposed In-Court and Out-of-Court Identifications charge, and the Courts findings on the factors have been incorporated into the following summary. Comparisons to the current charge have also been included where applicable. See Appendix A for the proposed
page6image11240 page6image11400 page6image11560 page6image11720 page6image11880 page6image12040
charges and Appendix B
for the current charges.
2
III. IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS CHARGE
A. First Two Paragraphs
1. Committees Discussion

Because the first two paragraphs conform to the current In-Court and Out-of Court Identifications charge, the only objection raised by the Committee concerned the inclusion of the phrase beyond a reasonable doubt in the last sentence of the second paragraph on page 2. That sentence in the original version stated:
You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. [Emphasis added.]
The Committee was divided on this issue. Some members asserted that this language is not included in the current charge, and therefore it should not be included in this charge either. They were of the view that since the first paragraph includes beyond a reasonable doubt it may be confusing to jurors to again include the burden in this paragraph.
Additionally, those members pointed out that this burden was stated again in the first sentence of the paragraph on page 3. That sentence stated:
To decide whether the identification testimony is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged, you should evaluate the testimony of the witness in light of the factors for considering credibility that I have already explained to you. [Emphasis added.]
page7image13320 page7image13480 page7image13640
3
Ultimately, the Committee agreed to delete the phrase upon which to conclude beyond a reasonable doubt in both of these sentences.
2. Recommended Language
The following was approved for the first two paragraphs:
(Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find this defendant guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed the crime. The defendant has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proven beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed it.
The State has presented the testimony of [insert name of witness who identified defendant]. You will recall that this witness identified the defendant in court as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. The State also presented testimony that on a prior occasion before this trial, this witness identified the defendant as the person who committed this offense [these offenses]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the witnesss identification of the defendant is reliable and believable, or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged.
page8image17888 page8image18048 page8image18208 page8image18368
1
86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981); State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v. Green,
page8image20768 page8image20928 page8image21088 page8image21248 page8image21408 page8image21568 page8image21728
4
B. Inclusion of References to Scientific Studies and Research
1. Committees Discussion
The subcommittee could not reach a consensus on whether specific references to the social science studies and scientific research included in Henderson should be noted in the third paragraph and in some of the factors, such as stress.
For example the third paragraph originally proposed by the subcommittee members in support of including these references stated:
Scientific research has amply demonstrated the dangers of mistake in human perception and identification. That research has focused on the nature of memory and the factors that affect the reliability of eyewitness identification evidence.
Those members stressed the importance of including the detailed description of the scientific findings and research found by the Court in Henderson. They were of the view that the jury should not only be told about the variables but also understand the science on how such variables can effect a witnesss memory.
On the other hand, the opposing members of the subcommittee stressed that the Committee needs to be aware that the social science research is probabilistic in that it cannot determine that a witness is right or wrong in his or her identification. However, those members acknowledged that certain factors may exist that should be explained to the jurors to help them determine the accuracy of the identification, and whether the State proved the identification beyond a reasonable doubt. Further, those members recognized that the jury
page9image13528 page9image13688
5
needs to know that the presence of certain factors may increase the chance of misidentification.
The Committee had a lengthy discussion on this issue. It was recognized that the cross-racial identification charge required in State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112 (1999), does not reference the social science studies although the charge is based upon those findings.
Most members agreed that the charge should not reference the social science studies or use the term scientific research. It was the general consensus that the charge should focus on the Courts findings as it relates to the jurys consideration of the reliability of the identification, and that the presence of certain factors may increase the chance of misidentification. For example, if the lighting is bad the charge does not need to state that the science shows...
Those members who initially opposed deleting these references ultimately withdrew their objections. Therefore, the Committee agreed to delete all references to the social science studies and scientific research in the charge.
page10image9256 page10image9416
6
C. Inclusion of Detailed Description on Memory and its Stages 1. Committees Discussion
The third and fifth paragraphs originally proposed by some members of the subcommittee included a detailed description on memory and its stages as set forth in Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 245-48. However, the subcommittee could not reach a consensus on including this language.
In particular, the original language in the third paragraph proposed by the supporting subcommittee members stated:
Eyewitness identification evidence must be scrutinized carefully. Scientific research has amply demonstrated the dangers of mistake in human perception and identification. That research has focused on the nature of memory and the factors that affect the reliability of eyewitness identification evidence. I will instruct you on the specific factors you should consider in this case in determining whether the eyewitness identification evidence is reliable.
Their fifth paragraph originally stated:
In evaluating the reliability of the identification, you should understand what science has demonstrated about how human memory works. Human memory is not at all like a video recording that a witness need only replay to remember what happened. Human memory is far more complex. Memory or the process of remembering consists of three stages: acquisition (the perception of the original event); retention (the period of time that passes between the event and the eventual recollection of a piece of information); and retrieval (the stage during which a person recalls stored information). At each of these stages, information that ultimately forms the memory can be distorted, contaminated, and even falsely imagined. Unlike a videotape, the witness does not perceive everything that occurs; rather, the witness constructs a memory based on pieces of information perceived at the time of the incident. In other words, the witness does not encode or record all the information in the same way that a videotape does. Additionally, whereas a
page11image17144 page11image17304 page11image17464
7
videotape remains intact, memory is malleable and can unknowingly be contaminated by a variety of factors.
The subcommittee members who proposed that language asserted that since the Court adopted the scientific research on memory, it should be included in the charge. Further those members were of the view that the first two sentences in the third paragraph go to the heart of the Henderson case. Additionally, those members asserted that the language in the fifth paragraph on how memory works was, in their view, the core of the scientific research in this area. They emphasized the importance of the jury understanding the sentence that memory is not like a video recording.
The opposing members of the subcommittee asserted that the fifth paragraph purported to instruct the jury on how memory works, which in their view was a subject beyond the scope of a jury instruction. They considered that paragraph essentially a lecture comparing human memory to videotape and filled with loaded words such as distorted, contaminated, false, and malleable. They were strongly opposed to including this language.
Those members suggested that the charge instead state:
Human beings do have the ability to recognize other people from past experiences and to identify them at a later time. But human memory is not foolproof.
One Committee member did not like the term foolproof. Other options were raised for this term but ultimately the Committee decided to keep foolproof.
The Committee held a lengthy discussion on whether to include the detailed description on memory and its stages. It was expressed that there is a
page12image14400
8
difference in including language in a court opinion and a charge. Overall, the general consensus was that the charge did not need to include a dissertation on memory and its stages. Further, the Committee was of the view that the charge should refrain from using too much description on memory.
Those members who initially supported including this language withdrew their objections.
Therefore, the Committee agreed for the effectiveness of the charge that there should be some preliminary information on memory. The Committee decided to include that memory is not like a video recording and that human memory is far more complex.
However, the Committee agreed to delete the remaining portion of the fifth paragraph concerning the detailed description on memory and its stages.
2. Recommended Third Paragraph
The Committee approved the following:
Eyewitness identification evidence must be scrutinized carefully. Human beings do have the ability to recognize other people from past experiences and to identify them at a later time. But human memory is not foolproof. Research has shown that human memory is not at all like a video recording that a witness need only replay to remember what happened. Human memory is far more complex.2 I will instruct you on specific factors you should consider in this case in determining whether the eyewitness identification evidence is reliable.
State v. Henderson, 208 N.J. 208, 245 (2011).
page13image13136
2
page13image13744 page13image13904
9
D. Fourth and Fifth Paragraphs on Evaluating the Identification
1. Committees Discussion
There were no objections to the language in these two paragraphs, which except for some minor changes is from the second and third paragraphs of the current charge.
page14image3088
3 4
2. Recommended Language
The Committee approved the following for these paragraphs:
In evaluating this identification, you should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, the witnesss ability to make those observations and perceptions, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.3
In evaluating the identifications, you should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification were based, and the witnesss ability to make those observations and perceptions. If you determine that the out-of-court identification is not reliable, you may still consider the witnesss in-court identification of the defendant if you find it to be reliable. Unless the in-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the out-of-court identification procedure, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issues of the reliability of both the in-court and out-of-court identifications is for you to decide.4
State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007).
page14image16160 page14image16320
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 229-232, 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1933-1935, 1940 (manner in which lineup or other identification procedure conducted relevant to reliability of out-of-court identification and in-court identification following out-of-court identification, and jurys credibility determinations).
page14image18760 page14image18920 page14image19080
10
E. Introductory Paragraph for the Estimator Factors
The charge first addresses the Estimator variables. These are the factors beyond the control of the criminal justice system. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 261.
Moreover, the Court explained:
They can include factors related to the incident, the witness, or the perpetrator. Estimator variables are equally capable of affecting an eyewitness ability to perceive and remember an event. Although the factors can be isolated and tested in lab experiments, they occur at random in the real world.
[ Ibid.]
It should be noted that like the current charge the term factors is used

rather than variables. In addition, except where noted below the paragraphs describing the factors are new to the charge.
1. Committees Discussion
This paragraph is the same as the current charge, except for the addition of the third sentence, which states, In addition you should consider the following factors that are related to the witness, the alleged perpetrator of the crime, and the criminal incident itself.
One area of disagreement among the subcommittee members was the use of may versus should in regards to the jurys consideration of the factors in this paragraph. The subcommittee members supporting the use of should argued that in their opinion the research based variables that affect reliability as discussed in Henderson require the fact finder to consider the factors that science has demonstrated affect reliability because they are based on research.
page15image13744 page15image13904 page15image14064 page15image14224 page15image14384 page15image14544
11
The subcommittee members who preferred the use of may explained that the use of should risks directing jury deliberations, while the use of may draws attention to the findings but permits the jury to decide whether they should be applied. Those subcommittee members emphasized that there is a danger of channeling the jurors discretion by the use of should rather than may.
The Committee was divided on this issue. It was expressed that the current charge uses should, and the Court did not recommend changing that language. By a close vote, the Committee agreed to use should.
2. Recommended Paragraph
The Committee approved the following:
To decide whether the identification testimony is sufficiently reliable evidence that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged, you should evaluate the testimony of the witness in light of the factors for considering credibility that I have already explained to you. In addition, you should consider the following factors that are related to the witness, the alleged perpetrator of the crime, and the criminal incident itself.5 In particular, you should consider [choose appropriate factors from one through six below]:
page16image10624
5
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 247.
page16image11784 page16image11944 page16image12104
12
F. The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention Factor
1. Committees Discussion
This language, except for some minor changes, is from the first two factors of the current charge on page 3.
2. Recommended Paragraph
The Committee approved the following for this factor:
(1) The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention: In evaluating the reliability of the identification, you should assess the witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense and the witnesss degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense. In making this assessment you should consider the following [choose appropriate factors from (a) through (i) below]:
13
G. Stress Factor
1. Courts Findings

In Henderson, the Court found that high levels of stress are likely to affect the reliability of eyewitness identifications. Id. at 262. However, the Court noted that There is no precise measure for what constitutes high stress, which must be assessed based on the facts presented in individual cases. Ibid.
2. Committees Discussion
The original version reviewed by the Committee stated:
Stress: Scientific research has proven that even under the best viewing conditions, highly stressful events can have a negative effect on memory and increase the risk of a mistaken identification. Therefore, you should consider the level of the witness stress and whether the stressful nature of the event distracted the witness or made it harder for him or her to identify the perpetrator.
The Committee first deleted the phrase Scientific research has proven in the first sentence.
The Committee then recognized the importance of the statement in Henderson that [e]ven under the best viewing conditions, high levels of stress can diminish an eyewitness ability to recall and make an accurate identification. Id. at 261. Therefore, the Committee agreed to revise the first sentence to conform to that statement.
The Committee next agreed to delete the phrase in the last sentence concerning the stressful nature of the event.
page18image12664 page18image12824 page18image12984 page18image13144 page18image13304
14
3. Recommended Paragraph
The paragraph approved by the Committee is as follows:
(a) Stress: Even under the best viewing conditions, high levels of stress can reduce an eyewitnesss ability to recall and make an accurate identification. Therefore, you should consider the level of the witnesss stress and whether that stress, if any, distracted the witness or made it harder for him or her to identify the perpetrator. 6
page19image4600
6
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 261-62.
page19image5760 page19image5920 page19image6080
15
H. Weapons Focus Factor 1. Courts Findings
For this factor, the Court explained that When a visible weapon is used during a crime, it can distract a witness and draw his or her attention away from the culprit. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 262.
Further, the Court stated:
  1. (1) Weapon focus can thus impair a witness ability to make a reliable identification and describe what the culprit looks like if the crime is of short duration.
  2. (2) Weapon focus can also affect a witness ability to describe a perpetrator.
  3. (3) The duration of the crime is also an important consideration.
    [ Id. at 262-63.]
2. Committees Discussion
The original version reviewed by the Committee stated:
Weapons Focus: You should consider whether a weapon was visible to the witness during the incident. The presence of a weapon can distract the witness and take the witness attention away from the perpetrators face. As a result, the presence of a visible weapon may reduce the reliability of a subsequent identification. In considering this factor, you should take into account the duration of the crime because the longer the duration of the event, the more time the witness may have to adapt to the presence of the weapon and focus on other details.
The Committee was mindful of the Courts findings in Henderson that when the interaction is brief, the presence of a visible weapon can affect the reliability of an identification and the accuracy of a witness description of the perpetrator. Id. at 263.
page20image15048 page20image15208 page20image15368 page20image15528 page20image15688 page20image15848
16
Further, the Committee agreed that this factor will not apply in every case where there is a visible weapon, i.e. it will only apply if the crime is of short duration. Therefore, the Committee decided to add that phrase at the end of the third sentence.
3. Recommended Paragraph
The Committee approved the following paragraph:
(b) Weapons Focus: You should consider whether the witness saw the weapon during the incident. The presence of a weapon can distract the witness and take the witnesss attention away from the perpetrators face. As a result, the presence of a visible weapon may reduce the reliability of a subsequent identification if the crime is of a short duration. In considering this factor, you should take into account the duration of the crime because the longer the duration of the event, the more time the witness may have to adapt to the presence of the weapon and focus on other details.7
page21image9368
7
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 262-63.
page21image10528 page21image10688 page21image10848
17
I. Duration Factor
1. Courts Findings

The Court found that Not surprisingly, the amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. Id. at 264.
Further, the Court noted that Whatever the threshold, studies have shown, and the Special Master found, that witnesses consistently tend to overestimate short durations, particularly where much was going on or the event was particularly stressful. Ibid.
2. Committees Discussion
The original language reviewed by the Committee for this factor stated:
Duration: The amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. A brief or fleeting observation is less likely to produce an accurate identification than a more prolonged exposure. In addition, scientific research has shown that the time estimate given by a witness may not always be accurate because witnesses tend to think events lasted longer than they actually did.
The Committee was mindful of the importance of including the statement in Henderson that There is no measure to determine exactly how long a view is needed to be able to make a reliable identification. Ibid. Therefore, the Committee agreed to add Although there is no minimum time required to make an accurate identification at the beginning of the second sentence. In addition, the phrase to the perpetrator was added at the end of that sentence.
page22image12896 page22image13056 page22image13216 page22image13376
18
3. Recommended Paragraph
The Committee approved the following paragraph:
(c) Duration: The amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. Although there is no minimum time required to make an accurate identification a brief or fleeting contact is less likely to produce an accurate identification than a more prolonged exposure to the perpetrator. In addition, the time estimate given by a witness may not always be accurate because witnesses tend to think events lasted longer than they actually did. 8
page23image5648
8
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 264.
page23image6808 page23image6968 page23image7128
19
J. Distance and Lighting Factors 1. Courts Findings
The Henderson Court found:
It is obvious that a person is easier to recognize when close by, and that clarity decreases with distance. We also know that poor lighting makes it harder to see well. Thus, greater distance between a witness and a perpetrator and poor lighting conditions can diminish the reliability of an identification.
[ Id. at 264.]
The Court also noted Research has shown that people have difficulty

estimating distances. Ibid.
2. Committees Discussion
The third sentence in the original version for the Distance factor stated In addition, scientific research has shown that a witness estimate of how far he was from the perpetrator may not always be accurate because people tend to have difficulty estimating distances. The only change made by the Committee to this paragraph was to delete the phrase scientific research has shown in that sentence.
With regards to the Lighting factor, the original language stated:
Lighting: Poor lighting makes it harder to see well, and therefore, poor lighting conditions can diminish the reliability of an identification.
The Committee considered the Courts findings that poor lighting conditions can diminish the reliability of an identification. Various alternatives were discussed for the term poor, but the Committee ultimately agreed on inadequate.
page24image13000 page24image13160 page24image13320
20
The Committee also decided to add You should consider the lighting conditions that were present at the time of the alleged crime in this case.
3. Recommended Paragraphs
The approved language states:
(d) Distance: A person is easier to recognize when close by. The greater the distance between an eyewitness and a perpetrator, the higher the risk of a mistaken identification. In addition, a witnesss estimate of how far he or she was from the perpetrator may not always be accurate because people tend to have difficulty estimating distances. 9
(e) Lighting: Inadequate lighting can reduce the reliability of an identification. You should consider the lighting conditions that were present at the time of the alleged crime in this case.10
page25image7632
9 10
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 264. Ibid.
page25image9080 page25image9240 page25image9400 page25image9560
21
K. Intoxication Factor
1. Courts Findings

The Court found that the level of intoxication can affect the reliability of an identification. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 265.
In support of this conclusion the Court noted the findings of the Special Master that the effects of alcohol on identification accuracy show that high levels of alcohol promote false identifications and that low alcohol intake produces fewer misidentifications than high alcohol intake. Ibid.
2. Committees Discussion
In crafting the language for this factor, the subcommittee could not reach a consensus. The version proposed by some members of the subcommittee stated:
Intoxication: An identification given by a witness who was highly impaired or intoxicated at the time of the incident tends to be more unreliable than an identification given by a sober witness.
The opposing members of the subcommittee pointed out that Henderson only referenced alcohol and not narcotics or prescription drugs. Those members noted that the description of this factor in Henderson does not include any findings or discussion on whether prescription drugs or narcotics have an effect on the identification and whether the jury should be advised on them. Moreover, they asserted that Henderson is not just tied to intoxication; rather the opinion ties the reliability of the identification to the level of intoxication.
page26image12368 page26image12528 page26image12688 page26image12848 page26image13008 page26image13168 page26image13328 page26image13488
22
The Committee first considered whether jurors would understand the term intoxication and whether to instead reference under the influence. The Committee could not reach a consensus on which term was better.
The Committee then noted the importance of the findings of the Special Master that high levels of alcohol promote false identifications, whereas low alcohol intake produces fewer misidentifications than high alcohol intake. Therefore, the Committee agreed to include that phrase rather than highly impaired or intoxicated.
Some Committee members asserted that the charge should include references to any evidence, including drugs that might have an effect on a witness such as alcohol or drugs. After a lengthy discussion, a majority of the Committee approved adding a sentence to the footnote, which would state that Although Henderson only addressed alcohol if there is evidence of impairment by drugs or other substances the parties may want to have the jury consider this issue.
After the Committee meeting, some members indicated that they were going to raise an issue at the next meeting that the Intoxication factor should be revisited because one of the Estimator variables that should be evaluated during a pre-trial hearing after suggestiveness has been shown is, [w]as the witness under the influence of alcohol or drugs. Therefore, they planned to suggest that this factor be revised to state a witness who is highly intoxicated and/or whether the witness was under the influence of alcohol or drugs. However, those members did not raise this issue at the next meeting, nor were there any
page27image13576
23
additional objections raised concerning this language in the final approved charge.
3. Recommended Paragraph
The language approved by the Committee states:
(f) Intoxication: An identification given by a witness with a high level of alcohol at the time of the incident tends to be more unreliable than an identification given by a witness who did not have a high level of alcohol.11
page28image4584
11
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 265. Although Henderson only addressed alcohol if there is evidence of impairment by drugs or other substances the parties may want to have the jury consider this issue.
page28image7128 page28image7288 page28image7448 page28image7608
24
L. Disguises/Changed Appearance Factor 1. Courts Findings
The Court stated, Disguises and changes in facial features can affect a witness ability to remember and identify a perpetrator. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 266. Further the Court recognized the Special Masters findings that [d]isguises (e.g., hats, sunglasses, masks) are confounding to witnesses and reduce the accuracy of identifications. Ibid.
Additionally, the Court found that disguises as simple as hats have been shown to reduce identification accuracy. Ibid. Moreover, the Court noted If facial features are altered between the time of the event and the identification procedure if for example, the culprit grows a beardthe accuracy of an identification may decrease. Ibid.
page29image6736 page29image6896 page29image7056 page29image7216 page29image7376 page29image7536 page29image7696 page29image7856
12 13
2. Committees Discussion
The Committee approved the language as proposed by the subcommittee.
3. Recommended Paragraph
The approved language states:
(g) Disguises/Changed Appearance: The perpetrators use of a disguise can affect a witnesss ability to both remember and identify the perpetrator. Disguises can be as simple as hats or sunglasses, as well as masks, and can reduce the accuracy of an identification.12 Similarly, if facial features are altered between the time of the event and a subsequent identification procedure, the accuracy of the identification may decrease.13
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 266. Ibid.
page29image14656 page29image14816 page29image14976 page29image15136
25
M. Prior Description of Perpetrator Factor 1. Committees Discussion
The subcommittee could not reach a consensus on the language for this factor. The paragraph proposed by some members of the subcommittee stated:
Prior Description of Perpetrator: Another important factor for your consideration is the accuracy of any description of the perpetrator that the witness gave prior to the identification procedure. Consider whether any description given prior to the identification procedure accurately matches the photo or person picked out during the procedure. Additionally, you should give careful consideration to whether the witness testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his or her prior description of the perpetrator. Finally, you should consider whether the prior description provided details or was just general in nature because a witness inability to provide a detailed description of the perpetrator may call into question the reliability of the witness identification
The opposing members of the subcommittee asserted that the sentence whether any description given prior to the identification procedure accurately matches the photo or person picked out during the procedure was not in Henderson.
The Committee discussed this issue and ultimately agreed to use the language in the current charge on page 3, which advises the jury that they may consider the accuracy of any description the witness gave prior to identifying the perpetrator. In addition, the Committee decided that the first sentence of the footnote in the current charge should be included in this paragraph, and to delete the rest of the footnote.
page30image14048
26
2. Recommended Paragraph
The language approved by the Committee states:
(2) Prior Description of Perpetrator: Another factor for your consideration is the accuracy of any description the witness gave before identifying the perpetrator. Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided details or was just general in nature, and whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator.
27
N. Confidence and Accuracy Factor 1. Courts Findings
With regards to highly confident witnesses, the Court recognized its conclusion in State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007), that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 254.
Further, the Henderson Court noted the Special Masters findings that eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy, but he acknowledged research showing that highly confident witnesses can make accurate identifications 90% of the time. Ibid.
2. Committees Discussion
The subcommittee could not reach a consensus on this paragraph. The version proposed by some members of the subcommittee stated:
Confidence and Accuracy: You heard testimony that (insert name of witness) made a statement at the time of his/her identification of defendant from the photo array/line-up concerning his/her level of certainty that the photograph he/she selected is in fact the person who committed the crime. A witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification. Although some scientific research has found that highly confident witnesses are more likely to make accurate identifications, eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy.
One Committee member expressed concerns with the sentence that Although some scientific research has found that highly confident witnesses are more likely to make accurate identifications, eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy. That member was concerned that this
page32image13984 page32image14144 page32image14304 page32image14464 page32image14624 page32image14784 page32image14944 page32image15104
28
statement bolsters the testimony of the States witness. The Committee initially considered deleting that sentence.
However, the subcommittee member who proposed that sentence explained that it was drafted to balance the language in Henderson that a confidence statement taken at the time of the out-of-court identification without confirmatory feedback and recorded would be relevant at the trial. In his view, if the statement was not recorded it could not be used at trial. However, it was expressed that a confidence statement is only relevant if it was made at the time of the identification.
After a lengthy discussion, the Committee decided to delete the phrase scientific in that sentence. In addition, the Committee agreed to delete the sentence that A witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.
page33image7784 page33image7944
14
3. Recommended Paragraph
The language approved by the Committee states:
(3) Confidence and Accuracy: You heard testimony that (insert name of witness) made a statement at the time of his/her identification of defendant from the photo array/line-up concerning his/her level of certainty that the photograph he/she selected is in fact the person who committed the crime. Although some research has found that highly confident witnesses are more likely to make accurate identifications, eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy.14
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 253-55.
page33image13824 page33image13984 page33image14144
29
O. Time Elapsed Factor
1. Courts Findings

With regards to memory decay, the Court stated:
Memories fade with time. And as the Special Master observed, memory decay is irreversible; memories never improve. As a result, delays between the commission of a crime and the time an identification is made can affect reliability. That basic principle is not in dispute.
[ See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 267.]
Further, the Court stated, In other words, the more time that passes, the

greater the possibility that a witness memory of a perpetrator will weaken. Ibid.
2. Committees Discussion
The first three sentences proposed by some members of the subcommittee stated:
Scientific evidence has proven that memories fade with time. This process is not reversible. Once a memory fades or decays it can never be recovered.
However, the subcommittee could not reach a consensus on including these sentences. The subcommittee members opposed to this language asserted that memory can be recalled.
In reviewing this paragraph, the Committee first deleted the reference to scientific evidence has proven that in the first sentence. The Committee then considered the statement in Henderson that researchers cannot pinpoint precisely when a persons recall becomes unreliable. Id. at 267.
After a lengthy discussion, the Committee agreed to retain the Memories fade with time sentence and delete the second and third sentences.
page34image13440 page34image13600 page34image13760 page34image13920 page34image14080 page34image14240 page34image14400
30
3. Recommended Paragraph
The language approved by the Committee states:
(4) Time Elapsed: Memories fade with time. As a result, delays between the commission of a crime and the time an identification is made can affect the reliability of the identification. In other words, the more time that passes, the greater the possibility that a witnesss memory of a perpetrator will weaken.15
page35image4256
15
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 267.
page35image5416 page35image5576 page35image5736
31
P. Cross-Racial Effects Factor 1. Courts Findings
With regards to cross-racial identification the Henderson Court citing to State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 120 (1999) explained:
A cross-racial identification occurs when an eyewitness is asked to identify a person of another race. Cromedy, supra, 158 N.J. at 120. In Cromedy, after citing multiple social science sources, this Court recognized that a witness may have more difficulty making a cross-racial identification. [citation omitted].
[Id. at 267.]
Further, the Court stated that [c]ross-racial recognition continues to be a factor that can affect the reliability of an identification. Ibid.
As to the charge for cross-racial identification, the Court advised:
In 1999, the Court in Cromedy directed that the charge be given only when . . . identification is a critical issue in the case, and an eyewitnesss cross-racial identification is not corroborated by other evidence giving it independent reliability. Cromedy, supra, 158 N.J. at 132. Since then, the additional research on own-race bias discussed in section VI.B.8, and the more complete record about eye-witness identification in general, justify giving the charge whenever cross-racial identification is in issue at trial.
[ Id. at 299.]
2. Committees Discussion
The original paragraph reviewed by the Committee stated:
Cross-Racial Effects: The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant is a significant factor that may influence the accuracy of the witness identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race. In fact,
page36image15416 page36image15576 page36image15736 page36image15896 page36image16056 page36image16216
32
cross-racial identifications have been proven to be more unreliable than same-race identifications.
The Committee first turned to the language in the current charge on page 6, and compared it to the proposed paragraph. The Committee agreed to use the language in the current charge, and to add in the footnote that this instruction must rather than should be given whenever there is a cross-racial identification.
3. Recommended Paragraph
The language approved by the Committee states:
(5) Cross-Racial Effects: The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race. 16
page37image8800
16
This instruction must be given whenever there is a cross-racial identification. Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 299 (modifying State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 132 (1999).
page37image11064 page37image11224 page37image11384 page37image11544 page37image11704
33
Q. Catch-all Factor
1. OriginalFactorsonChildWitnessesandOwn-Age Bias
Some members of the subcommittee proposed including separate factors on Child Witnesses and Own-Age Bias, which stated the following:
Child Witnesses: Identifications made by young children must be scrutinized carefully. Child eyewitnesses under the age of thirteen are more likely to make incorrect identifications than adults.
Own-Age Bias: Scientific evidence has shown that witnesses are better at recognizing people of their own age than people of other ages.
However, the subcommittee could not reach a consensus on whether the Court made specific findings on these factors, and whether they should be included in the charge.
In particular, the subcommittee members opposed to including these factors asserted that Henderson did not find that identifications made by young children must be scrutinized carefully. Further, those members asserted that the Special Masters finding was that [a] witnesss age ... bears on the reliability of an identification. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 265. Moreover, they pointed out that the research showed that children under thirteen were more likely to make incorrect identifications than adults when they view target-absent line-ups. Ibid.
As to the Own-Age Bias factor, the Committee considered the testimony of Dr. Penrod that witnesses are better at recognizing people of [their] own age than... people of other ages and that this may appear in studies that use college-age students as targets. Id. at 265-66. Additionally, it was asserted that
page38image13688 page38image13848 page38image14008 page38image14168 page38image14328 page38image14488 page38image14648
34
Henderson also referenced that Perhaps people should only use age as a factor in deciding whether to believe an eyewitness if there is a large age difference between the witness and the suspect. Id. at 266.
The Committee initially agreed to delete the first sentence in the Child Witnesses factor that Identifications made by young children must be scrutinized carefully. However, it was then expressed that the second sentence was not an accurate statement.
After further discussion, the Committee considered whether to have a Catch-all factor at the end of the section on Estimator variables, similar to factor 9 in the current charge, to incorporate the Courts findings that characteristics like a witness age...can affect the reliability of an identification. Id. at 265. Factor 9 in the current charge states that the Jury should be charged on any other relevant factor present in the case.
Most members agreed to include the Catch-all factor, and delete the Child Witnesses and Own-Age Bias factors. Those members that were opposed to deleting these factors did not renew their objections when the charges were ultimately approved.
2. RecommendedParagraph
The language approved by the Committee states:
(6) Catch-all Factor: You may also consider the age of the identifying witness and any other relevant characteristics of the witness.
page39image12144 page39image12304 page39image12464
35
R. Inconsistent Statements Factor 1. Committees Discussion
The original draft included an Inconsistent Statements factor, stated:
Inconsistent Statements: In evaluating the identification, you should consider any discrepancies or inconsistencies between the witness trial testimony and the out-of-court identification(s).
(The footnote at the end of this paragraph cited to State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118 (App. Div. 2000)).
which
page40image4800 page40image4960 page40image5120
Initially, the Committee approved the language in this factor, which is from factor 6 of the current charge.
However, upon reviewing the final version of the charge, the Committee decided that this factor was too repetitive with the Prior Description of Perpetrator factor. Therefore, the Committee agreed to delete the Inconsistent Statements factor.
36
S. Introductory Paragraph for the Systems Factors
These are factors such as lineup procedures, which are within the control of the criminal justice system. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 247.
1. Committees Discussion
There were no changes made by the Committee to the language proposed by the subcommittee.
page41image3688 page41image3848 page41image4008 page41image4168
2. Recommended Paragraph
The approved language states:
In evaluating the reliability of a witnesss identification, you should also consider the circumstances under which any out-of-court identification was made, and whether or not it was the result of a suggestive procedure, including everything done or said by law enforcement to the witness during the identification process. You should consider the following factors: [Charge if appropriate]:17
page41image8352
17
The following factors consist of the system ... variables ... for which [the Court] found scientific support that is generally accepted by experts. Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 298-99.
page41image10736 page41image10896 page41image11056
37
T. Lineup Composition and Fillers Factors 1. Courts Findings
The Court found:
The way that a live or photo lineup is constructed can also affect the reliability of an identification. Properly constructed lineups test a witness memory and decrease the chance that a witness is simply guessing.
[ See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 251.]
The Court listed the following features that can affect the construction of a fair lineup:
  1. (1) First, the Special Master found that mistaken identifications are more likely to occur when the suspect stands out from other members of a live or photo lineup... As a result, a suspect should be included in a lineup comprised of look-alikes. The reason is simple: an array of look-alikes forces witnesses to examine their memory. In addition, a biased lineup may inflate a witness confidence in the identification because the selection process seemed easy.
  2. (2) Second, lineups should include a minimum number of fillers. The greater the number of choices, the more likely the procedure will serve as a reliable test of the witness ability to distinguish the culprit from an innocent person. As Dr. Wells testified, no magic number exists, but there appears to be general agreement that a minimum of five fillers should be used.
  3. (3) Third, based on the same reasoning, lineups should not feature more than one suspect. As the Special Master found, if multiple suspects are in the lineup, the reliability of a positive identification is difficult to assess, for the possibility of lucky guesses is magnified.
    [ Id. at 251-52.]
page42image15536 page42image15696 page42image15856 page42image16016 page42image16176
38
The Court then stated, The record is unclear as to whether the use of fillers that match a witness pre-lineup description is more reliable than fillers that resemble an actual suspect (to the extent there is a difference between the two). Id. at 252.
The Court advised that:
[C]ourts should consider whether a lineup is poorly constructed when evaluating the admissibility of an identification. When appropriate, jurors should be told that poorly constructed or biased lineups can affect the reliability of an identification and enhance a witness confidence.
[ Ibid.]
2. Committees Discussion
The Committee did not make any changes to the language proposed by the subcommittee for these factors.
page43image7160 page43image7320 page43image7480
18 19
3. Recommended Paragraphs
The approved language states:
(1) Lineup Composition: A suspect should not stand out from other members of the lineup. The reason is simple: an array of look-alikes forces witnesses to examine their memory. In addition, a biased lineup may inflate a witnesss confidence in the identification because the selection process seemed so easy to the witness.18 It is, of course, for you to determine whether the lineup was biased or not and whether the composition of the lineup had any affect on the reliability of the identification.
(2) Fillers: Lineups should include a number of possible choices for the witness, commonly referred to as fillers. The greater the number of choices, the more likely the procedure will serve as a reliable test of the witnesss memory. A minimum of six persons or photos should be included in the lineup.19
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 251. Ibid.
page43image16824 page43image16984 page43image17144 page43image17304
39
U. Multiple Viewings Factor 1. Courts Findings
The Court stated:
Viewing a suspect more than once during an investigation can affect the reliability of the later identification. The problem, as the Special Master found, is that successive views of the same person can make it difficult to know whether the later identification stems from a memory of the original event or a memory of the earlier identification procedure.
[ See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 255.]
Additionally, the Court noted:
It is typical for eyewitnesses to look through mugshot books in search of a suspect. Investigations may also involve multiple identification procedures. Based on the record, there is no impact on the reliability of the second identification procedure when a picture of the suspect was not present in photographs examined earlier.
[ Ibid.]
The Court further explained that Multiple identification procedures that involve more than one viewing of the same suspect, though, can create a risk of mugshot exposure and mug-shot commitment. Ibid.
  1. (1) Mugshot exposure is when a witness initially views a set of photos and makes no identification, but then selects someone -- who had been depicted in the earlier photos -- at a later identification procedure.
  2. (2) Mugshot commitment occurs when a witness identifies a photo that is then included in a later lineup procedure. Studies have shown that once witnesses identify an innocent person from a mug-shot, a significant number then reaffirm[] their false identification in a later lineup -- even if the actual target is present.
    [ Id. at 255-56. ]
page44image15632 page44image15792 page44image15952 page44image16112 page44image16272 page44image16432 page44image16592
40
The Court concluded, Both mugshot exposure and mugshot commitment can affect the reliability of the witness ultimate identification and create a greater risk of misidentification. [ Id. at 256.]
2. Committees Discussion
It was recognized by the Committee that while there is language in Henderson advising that mugshot exposure and mugshot commitment can affect the reliability of the identification, the word mugshot should not be stated to the jury. Therefore, the Committee revised the end of the footnote to state that the jury should be instructed on the concepts implicated by those terms without using the word mugshot. In addition, the Committee agreed to include a reference to the model jury charge, Identity-Police Photos.
page45image6688 page45image6848
3. Recommended Paragraph
The approved language states:
(3) Multiple Viewing: When a witness views a person in multiple identification procedures, the witnesss memory of the actual perpetrator can be replaced by the witnesss memory of the person seen in the multiple procedures. In this way, when a witness views an innocent suspect in multiple identification procedures, the risk of mistaken identification is increased. You may consider whether the witness viewed the suspect multiple times and, if so, whether viewing the suspect in multiple procedures affected the reliability of the identification. 20
page45image12424
20
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 255-56. If either mugshot exposure (no identification in first lineup/photo array, later identification of someone in first procedure in second lineup/photo array) or mugshot commitment (selection of person in lineup who was identified in previous photo array) are part of the evidence, the jury should be instructed on the concepts implicated by those terms without using the word mugshot. See model charge on Identity-Police Photos.
page45image16864 page45image17024 page45image17184 page45image17344
41
V. Showups Factor
1. Courts Findings

The Henderson Court explained:
Showups are essentially single-person lineups: a single suspect is presented to a witness to make an identification. Showups often occur at the scene of a crime soon after its commission.
[ Id. at 259.] Further, the Court stated:
By their nature, showups are suggestive and cannot be performed blind or double-blind. Nonetheless, as the Special Master found, the risk of misidentification is not heightened if a showup is conducted immediately after the witnessed event, ideally within two hours because the benefits of a fresh memory seem to balance the risks of undue suggestion.
[ Ibid.]
The Court recognized that We have previously found showups to be inherently suggestive, see Herrera, supra, 187 N.J. at 504, 902 A.2d 177, and other states have limited the admissibility of showup identifications. Ibid.
Therefore, the Court concluded:
Thus, the record casts doubt on the reliability of showups conducted more than two hours after an event, which present a heightened risk of misidentification. As with lineups, showup administrators should instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the culprit and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification. That said, lineups are a preferred identification procedure because we continue to believe that showups, while sometimes necessary, are inherently suggestive. See Herrera, supra, 187 N.J. at 504, 902 A.2d 177.
[ Id. at 261.]
page46image14536 page46image14696 page46image14856 page46image15016 page46image15176
42
2. Committees Discussion
The original paragraph reviewed by the Committee stated:
Showups: In this case, the witness identified the defendant during a showup, that is, defendant was the only person shown to the witness at that time. Sometimes it is necessary for the police to conduct a showup, or one- on-one identification procedure, even though such a procedure is suggestive in nature. Although with immediate showups the benefits of a fresh memory may balance the risks of undue suggestion, showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk of misidentification. Also, police officers should instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification. If you find that the showup in this case was conducted more than two hours after the perpetrator was last seen by the witness, and/or if you find that the person conducting the lineup failed to instruct the witness that the person he/she was about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime, and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification, you may consider whether this/these factor(s) affected the accuracy of the identification.
The subcommittee members explained that in crafting this paragraph they strived to balance the language in Henderson that showups are inherently suggestive and that there is a heightened risk of misidentification. They also pointed out that this paragraph should be charged in every case in which there is a showup procedure.
The Committee had a lengthy discussion on this factor. One member was concerned with the proposed language, which in his opinion did not leave any room for whether the suggestiveness of the procedure should be considered within the first two hours. That member said that if the showup is outside two hours the jury can consider whether these factors weigh against the accuracy of
page47image17712
43
the identification, and not just that it affected the accuracy of the identification. Therefore, that member suggested that the end of this paragraph state that You may consider whether these factors weigh against the accuracy of the identification.
The Committee considered the Special Masters findings that the risk of misidentification is not heightened if a showup is conducted immediately after the witnessed event, ideally within two hours because the benefits of a fresh memory seem to balance the risks of undue suggestion. Id. at 259. Further, the Committee noted the statement in Henderson that the reliability of showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk of misidentification. Id. at 261.
The Committee then expressed the importance of explaining the two hours difference to the jury: (1) if the showup is done within the first two hours and the witness is given the appropriate instructions this may balance the risks of an increased level of suggestiveness; (2) if the showup is done after two hours then there is a heightened risk of misidentification; and (3) that they must consider whether the identification was reliable or the result of an unduly suggestive procedure.
A discussion then ensued on the use of immediate in the third sentence that Although with immediate showups the benefits of a fresh memory may balance the risks of undue suggestion, showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk of misidentification. It was expressed that Henderson does not define immediately but instead the Special Master referred to within two hours. The Committee could not reach a consensus on
page48image14304 page48image14464 page48image14624 page48image14784
44
this issue. Therefore, the Committee agreed to delete with immediate showups in that sentence.
The Committee also revised the next sentence stating Also, police officers should instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification to state must instruct and not should instruct.
It was then asserted that the charge does not include any language on what the jury should consider when the identification is done properly. For example, the jury is not told how to consider a showup that was conducted within ten minutes, and the appropriate instructions were given. Some members were strongly opposed to including such an instruction in the charge. They argued that Henderson states, by their nature, showups are suggestive. Id. at 259. Further, those member noted the Special Masters statement that the benefits of fresh memory seem to balance the risks of undue suggestion was hardly a positive statement on this procedure.
It was then expressed by one of the subcommittee members the importance of including the phrase Even though such a procedure is suggestive in nature to maintain the paragraphs balance. The Committee agreed to begin the second sentence with that phrase.
The Committee then agreed to delete the last sentence and revise it to the following:
In determining whether the identification is reliable or the result of undue suggestiveness procedure, you should consider how much time elapsed after the perpetrator was last seen by the
page49image14096 page49image14256
45
witness, and whether the appropriate instructions were given to the witness.
3. Recommended Paragraph
The Committee approved the following:
[CHARGE IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THERE IS A SHOWUP PROCEDURE]
(4) Showups: In this case, the witness identified the defendant during a showup, that is, defendant was the only person shown to the witness at that time. Even though such a procedure is suggestive in nature, sometimes it is necessary for the police to conduct a showup, or one-on-one identification procedure. Although the benefits of a fresh memory may balance the risk of undue suggestion, showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk of misidentification. Also, police officers must instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification. In determining whether the identification is reliable or the result of undue suggestive procedure, you should consider how much time elapsed after the perpetrator was last seen by the witness, and whether the appropriate instructions were given to the witness. 21
page50image11144
21
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 259-61
page50image12304 page50image12464 page50image12624
46
W. Identification Lineup Procedure
1. Lineup Administrator Factor

a. Courts Findings
With regards to the lineup procedure, the Court stated, An identification may be unreliable if the lineup procedure is not administered in double-blind or blind fashion. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 248.
Further, the Court distinguished the two methods of conducting the lineup:
(1) Double-blind administrators do not know who the actual suspect is. Its purpose is to prevent an administrator from intentionally or unintentionally influencing a witness identification decision;
(2) Blind administrators are aware of that information but shield themselves from knowing where the suspect is located in the lineup or photo array.
[Ibid.]
The Court further pointed out that Research has shown that lineup administrators familiar with the suspect may leak that information by consciously or unconsciously communicating to witnesses which lineup member is the suspect. Ibid.
Moreover, the Court added:
The consequences are clear: a non-blind lineup procedure can affect the reliability of a lineup because even the best- intentioned, non-blind administrator can act in a way that inadvertently sways an eyewitness trying to identify a suspect. An ideal lineup administrator, therefore, is someone who is not investigating the particular case and does not know who the suspect is.
[ Id. at 249.]
page51image13088 page51image13248 page51image13408 page51image13568 page51image13728 page51image13888 page51image14048
47
The Court further stated:
The State understandably notes that police departments, no matter their size, have limited resources, and those limits can make it impractical to administer lineups double-blind in all cases. An alternative technique, which Dr. Wells referred to as the envelope method, helps address that challenge. It relies on single-blind administration: an officer who knows the suspects identity places single lineup photographs into different envelopes, shuffles them, and presents them to the witness. The officer/administrator then refrains from looking at the envelopes or pictures while the witness makes an identification. This blinding technique is cost-effective and can be used when resource constraints make it impractical to perform double-blind administration.
[ Id. at 249-50.]
The Court concluded that the failure to perform blind lineup procedures

can increase the likelihood of misidentification. Id. at 250.
b. Committees Discussion
The subcommittee in crafting this paragraph agreed that this factor should be charged in every case where the police conduct an identification lineup procedure. In addition, they included an optional paragraph to address circumstances where the police do not use a blind administrator.
The Committee approved the language as proposed by the subcommittee.
c. Recommended Paragraph
The approved language states:
page52image12112 page52image12272
48
[CHARGE (a) and (b) IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THE POLICE CONDUCT AN IDENTIFICATION LINEUP PROCEDURE]22
In determining the reliability of the identification, you should also consider whether the identification procedure was properly conducted.
(a) Double-blind: A lineup administrator who knows which lineup member is the suspect may intentionally or unintentionally convey this knowledge to the witness, thereby increasing the chance that the witness will identify the suspect even if the suspect is innocent. For this reason, whenever feasible, lineups and photo-spreads should be conducted by an officer who does not know the identity of the suspect.23
[CHARGE IF BLIND ADMINISTRATOR IS NOT USED]
If a police officer who does not know the suspects identity is not available, then the officer should use a method that does not allow him/her to see the photos as the witness looks at them. In this case, it is alleged that the person who presented the lineup [did/did not] know the identity of the suspect.
[In this case, it is alleged that a police officer who did not know the suspects identity was not available, and the police did/did not compensate for that by conducting a procedure where the officer did not see the photos as the witness looked at them.]
page53image11968
22
and estimator variables.)
23
To help jurors weigh that evidence, they must be told about relevant factors and their effect on reliability. Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 219 (asking the Criminal Practice Committee and the Committee on Model Criminal Jury Charges to draft proposed revisions to this charge and address various system
page53image15808 page53image15968 page53image16128
Id. at 248-50.
page53image16816
49
2. InstructionsFactor
a. Courts Findings

With regards to Pre-identification Instructions, the Court stated:
Identification procedures should begin with instructions to the witness that the suspect may or may not be in the lineup or array and that the witness should not feel compelled to make an identification. There is a broad consensus for that conclusion.
[ Id. at 250.]
The Court found that the failure to give proper pre-lineup instructions can increase the risk of misidentification. Ibid.
b. Committees Discussion
Like the Lineup Administrator factor, the Committee agreed that this factor should also be charged in every case where the police conduct an identification lineup procedure.
page54image7232 page54image7392 page54image7552
24 25
The Committee approved the language as proposed by the subcommittee.
c. Recommended Paragraph
The approved language states:
You may consider this factor when you consider the circumstances under which the identification was made, and when you evaluate the overall reliability of the identification.24
(b) Instructions: You should consider what was or what was not said to the witness prior to viewing a photo array.25 Identification procedures should begin with instructions to the witness that the perpetrator may or may not be in the array and that the witness should not feel compelled to make an identification. The failure to give this instruction can increase the risk of misidentification. If you find that the police [did/did
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 248-50.
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995).

page54image16016 page54image16176 page54image16336 page54image16496 page54image16656 page54image16816
50
not] give this instruction to the witness, you may take this factor into account when evaluating the identification evidence.26
page55image1792
26
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 250.
page55image2952 page55image3112 page55image3272
51
3. Feedback Factor
a. Courts Findings

The Court stated, Information received by witnesses both before and after an identification can affect their memory. Id. at 253.
The Court explained:
Confirmatory or post-identification feedback presents the same risks. It occurs when police signal to eyewitnesses that they correctly identified the suspect. That confirmation can reduce doubt and engender a false sense of confidence in a witness. Feedback can also falsely enhance a witness recollection of the quality of his or her view of an event.
[ Ibid.]
page56image5864 page56image6024
Therefore, the Court concluded, feedback affects the reliability identification in that it can distort memory, create a false sense of confidence, and alter a witness report of how he or she viewed an event. Id. at 255.
Moreover, based upon the record, the Court found:
[N]on-State actors like co-witnesses and other sources of information can affect the independent nature and reliability of identification evidence and inflate witness confidence -- in the same way that law enforcement feedback can. As a result, law enforcement officers should instruct witnesses not to discuss the identification process with fellow witnesses or obtain information from other sources.
[ Id. at 271.]
b. Committees Discussion
The only change the Committee made to the paragraph on Feedback proposed by the subcommittee was to revise the bracketed text above this factor to state [CHARGE IF FEEDBACK IS AN ISSUE IN THE CASE] rather than stating [CHARGE IF SUPPORTED BY THE EVIDENCE].
of an
page56image15424 page56image15584
52
c. Recommended Paragraph
The following was approved:
[CHARGE IF FEEDBACK IS AN ISSUE IN THE CASE]
(c) Feedback: Feedback occurs when police or other witnesses to an event signal to eyewitnesses that they correctly identified the suspect. That confirmation poses a risk of endangering a false sense of confidence in a witness. Feedback can also present a risk of falsely enhancing a witnesss recollection of the quality of his or her view of an event. It is for you to determine whether or not the recollection of the witness was affected by feedback or whether the recollection instead reflects the accurate perceptions of the witness during the event.27
page57image7000
27
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 253-55; see also Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781 n.2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n.8 (Utah 1986)).
page57image9464 page57image9624 page57image9784 page57image9944 page57image10104 page57image10264 page57image10424 page57image10584 page57image10744 page57image10904
53
X. Concluding Paragraphs
1. Committees Discussion

The Committee approved the final three paragraphs as proposed by the subcommittee.
The first paragraph includes the language in the current charge in the last identification procedure factor on page 5. However, the last sentence was added to state Such information can affect the independent nature and reliability of a witnesss identification and inflate witness confidence in that identification. In addition the footnote now cites to State v. Chen, 208 N.J. 307(2011), instead of State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (2006).
The first sentence of the next paragraph, except for some minor changes, is from factor 8 of the current charge. The next two sentences are new:
Keep in mind that the presence of any single factor or combination of factor(s), however, is not an indication that a particular witness is incorrect. Instead, you may consider the factor or factor(s) which I have discussed as you assess all of the circumstances of the case, including all of the testimony and documentary evidence in determining whether a particular identification made by a witness is accurate and thus worthy of your consideration as you determine whether the State has met its burden to prove identification beyond a reasonable doubt.
The subsequent sentences beginning with Unless the in-court and out-of- court identifications... to the end of the charge are from the current charge.
2. Recommended Paragraphs
The Committee approved the following:
page58image13496 page58image13656 page58image13816 page58image13976
54
You may consider whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions, or identifications given by other witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other information or influence, that may have affected the independence of his/her identification.28 Such information can affect the independent nature and reliability of a witnesss identification and inflate witness confidence in that identification.
You are also free to consider any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case that you consider relevant to your determination whether the identifications were reliable. Keep in mind that the presence of any single factor or combination of factor(s), however, is not an indication that a particular witness is incorrect. Instead, you may consider the factor or factor(s) which I have discussed as you assess all of the circumstances of the case, including all of the testimony and documentary evidence in determining whether a particular identification made by a witness is accurate and thus worthy of your consideration as you determine whether the State has met its burden to prove identification beyond a reasonable doubt. Unless the in-court and out-of-court identifications resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the in-court and/or out-of-court identification procedures, they should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of an identification is for you to decide.
If, after consideration of all of the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after consideration of all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
page59image16896
28
State v. Chen, 208 N.J. 307 (2011).
page59image18096 page59image18256
55
Respectfully Submitted,
Honorable Samuel D. Natal, Chair Honorable Greta Gooden-Brown, Vice-Chair Honorable Robert C. Billmeier
Honorable Joseph C. Cassini, III
Honorable Liliana S. DeAvila-Silebi Honorable Marianne Espinosa
Honorable Thomas M. Moore
Honorable Ronald L. Reisner
Honorable Lisa Rose
Honorable Francis B. Schultz
Honorable Michael A. Toto
Joseph E. Krakora, Public Defender Deborah Bartolomey, Esq.
Robert J. Brass, Esq.
Annmarie Cozzi, Esq.
Catherine A. Foddai, Esq.
Mark H. Friedman, Esq.
Christine H. Kim, Esq.
Richard S. Lehrich, Esq.
John K. McNamara, Esq.
Alison S. Perrone, Esq.
Kevin G. Walsh, Esq.
Jack L. Weinberg, Esq.
Gwendolyn J. Williams, Esq.
Michael J. Williams, Esq.

Staff: Maria Pogue, Esq.
56
APPENDIX A PROPOSED CHARGES
Page 1 of 10 Revised 1/9/12
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS
(Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find this defendant guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed the crime. The defendant has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proven beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed it.
The State has presented the testimony of [insert name of witness who identified defendant]. You will recall that this witness identified the defendant in court as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. The State also presented testimony that on a prior occasion before this trial, this witness identified the defendant as the person who committed this offense [these offenses]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the witnesss identification of the defendant is reliable and
page62image14144 page62image14304 page62image14464 page62image14624
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 2 of 10
believable, or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged.
Eyewitness identification evidence must be scrutinized carefully. Human beings do have the ability to recognize other people from past experiences and to identify them at a later time. But human memory is not foolproof. Research has shown that human memory is not at all like a video recording that a witness need only replay to remember what happened. Human memory is far more complex.2 I will instruct you on specific factors you should consider in this case in determining whether the eyewitness identification evidence is reliable.
In evaluating this identification, you should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, the witnesss ability to make those observations and perceptions, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.3
In evaluating the identifications, you should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification were based, and the witnesss ability to make
page63image13576 page63image13736 page63image13896
1
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v.
page63image15416 page63image15576 page63image15736 page63image15896
Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981); State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119
(App. Div. 1996).
page63image17360 page63image17520 page63image17680 page63image17840
2 3
State v. Henderson, 208 N.J. 208, 245 (2011). State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007).
page63image19648 page63image19808 page63image19968 page63image20128
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 3 of 10
those observations and perceptions. If you determine that the out-of-court identification is not reliable, you may still consider the witnesss in-court identification of the defendant if you find it to be reliable. Unless the in-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the out-of-court identification procedure, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issues of the reliability of both the in-court and out-of-court identifications is for you to decide.4
To decide whether the identification testimony is sufficiently reliable evidence that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged, you should evaluate the testimony of the witness in light of the factors for considering credibility that I have already explained to you. In addition, you should consider the following factors that are related to the witness, the alleged perpetrator of the crime, and the criminal incident itself.5 In particular, you should consider [choose appropriate factors from one through six below]:
page64image10008 page64image10168
(1)
The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention: In evaluating the reliability of the identification, you should assess the witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense and the witnesss degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense. In making this assessment you should consider the following [choose appropriate factors from (a) through (i) below]:
(a) Stress: Even under the best viewing conditions, high levels of stress can reduce an eyewitnesss ability to recall and make an accurate identification. Therefore, you should consider the level of the witnesss stress and whether
page64image16192
4
identification, and jurys credibility determinations).
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 229-232, 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1933-1935, 1940 (manner in which lineup or other identification procedure conducted relevant to reliability of out-of-court identification and in-court identification following out-of-court
page64image19192 page64image19352 page64image19512
5
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 247.
page64image20672 page64image20832 page64image20992
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 4 of 10
page65image1456 page65image1616
that stress, if any, distracted the witness or made it harder for him or her to identify the perpetrator.6
(b) Weapons Focus: You should consider whether the witness saw the weapon during the incident. The presence of a weapon can distract the witness and take the witnesss attention away from the perpetrators face. As a result, the presence of a visible weapon may reduce the reliability of a subsequent identification if the crime is of a short duration. In considering this factor, you should take into account the duration of the crime because the longer the duration of the event, the more time the witness may have to adapt to the presence of the weapon and focus on other details.7
(c) Duration: The amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. Although there is no minimum time required to make an accurate identification a brief or fleeting contact is less likely to produce an accurate identification than a more prolonged exposure to the perpetrator. In addition, the time estimate given by a witness may not always be accurate because witnesses tend to think events lasted longer than they actually did.8
(d) Distance: A person is easier to recognize when close by. The greater the distance between an eyewitness and a perpetrator, the higher the risk of a mistaken identification. In addition, a witnesss estimate of how far he or she was from the perpetrator may not always be accurate because people tend to have difficulty estimating distances.9
(e) Lighting: Inadequate lighting can reduce the reliability of an identification. You should consider the lighting conditions that were present at the time of the alleged crime in this case.10
(f) Intoxication: An identification given by a witness with a high level of alcohol at the time of the incident tends to be more unreliable than an identification given by a witness who did not have a high level of alcohol. 11
(g) Disguises/Changed Appearance: The perpetrators use of a disguise can affect a witnesss ability to both remember and identify the perpetrator.
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 261-62. Id. at 262-63.
Id. at 264.
Ibid.

Ibid.
Id. at 265. Although Henderson only addressed alcohol if there is evidence of impairment by drugs or other substances the parties may want to have the jury consider this issue.
page65image22760
6 7 8 9 10 11
page65image24088 page65image24248 page65image24408 page65image24568 page65image24728 page65image24888 page65image25048 page65image25208 page65image25368
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 5 of 10
Disguises can be as simple as hats or sunglasses, as well as masks, and can reduce the accuracy of an identification.12 Similarly, if facial features are altered between the time of the event and a subsequent identification procedure, the accuracy of the identification may decrease.13
(2) Prior Description of Perpetrator: Another factor for your consideration is the accuracy of any description the witness gave before identifying the perpetrator. Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided details or was just general in nature, and whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator.
(3) Confidence and Accuracy: You heard testimony that (insert name of witness) made a statement at the time of his/her identification of defendant from the photo array/line-up concerning his/her level of certainty that the photograph he/she selected is in fact the person who committed the crime. Although some research has found that highly confident witnesses are more likely to make accurate identifications, eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy.14
(4) Time Elapsed: Memories fade with time. As a result, delays between the commission of a crime and the time an identification is made can affect the reliability of the identification. In other words, the more time that passes, the greater the possibility that a witnesss memory of a perpetrator will weaken.15
(5) Cross-Racial Effects: The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.16
(6) Catch-all Factor: You may also consider the age of the identifying witness and any other relevant characteristics of the witness.
page66image18808 page66image18968 page66image19128
12 13 14 15 16
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 266. Ibid.
Id. at 253-55.
Id. at 267.

page66image21456 page66image21616 page66image21776 page66image21936 page66image22096 page66image22256
This instruction must be given whenever there is a cross-racial identification. Id. at 299 (modifying State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 132 (1999)).
page66image23848 page66image24008 page66image24168
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 6 of 10
In evaluating the reliability of a witnesss identification, you should also consider the circumstances under which any out-of-court identification was made, and whether or not it was the result of a suggestive procedure, including everything done or said by law enforcement to the witness during the identification process. You should consider the following factors: [Charge if appropriate]:17
(1) Lineup Composition: A suspect should not stand out from other members of the lineup. The reason is simple: an array of look-alikes forces witnesses to examine their memory. In addition, a biased lineup may inflate a witnesss confidence in the identification because the selection process seemed so easy to the witness.18 It is, of course, for you to determine whether the lineup was biased or not and whether the composition of the lineup had any affect on the reliability of the identification.
(2) Fillers: Lineups should include a number of possible choices for the witness, commonly referred to as fillers. The greater the number of choices, the more likely the procedure will serve as a reliable test of the witnesss memory. A minimum of six persons or photos should be included in the lineup.19
(3) Multiple Viewing: When a witness views a person in multiple identification procedures, the witnesss memory of the actual perpetrator can be replaced by the witnesss memory of the person seen in the multiple procedures. In this way, when a witness views an innocent suspect in multiple identification procedures, the risk of mistaken identification is increased. You may consider whether the witness viewed the suspect multiple times and, if so, whether viewing the suspect in multiple procedures affected the reliability of the identification. 20
page67image15336 page67image15496 page67image15656
17
The following factors consist of the system ... variables ... for which [the
Court] found scientific support that is generally accepted by experts. Henderson, supra,
208 N.J. at 298-99.
page67image18360 page67image18520
18 19 20
Id. at 251
Ibid.
Id. at 255-56. If either mugshot exposure (no identification in first lineup/photo

page67image20576 page67image20736 page67image20896 page67image21056
array, later identification of someone in first procedure in second lineup/photo array) or mugshot commitment (selection of person in lineup who was identified in previous photo array) are part of the evidence, the jury should be instructed on the concepts implicated by those terms without using the word mugshot. See model charge on Identity-Police Photos.
page67image24240
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 7 of 10
[CHARGE IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THERE IS A SHOWUP PROCEDURE]
(4) Showups: In this case, the witness identified the defendant during a showup, that is, defendant was the only person shown to the witness at that time. Even though such a procedure is suggestive in nature, sometimes it is necessary for the police to conduct a showup, or one-on-one identification procedure. Although the benefits of a fresh memory may balance the risk of undue suggestion, showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk of misidentification. Also, police officers must instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification. In determining whether the identification is reliable or the result of undue suggestive procedure, you should consider how much time elapsed after the perpetrator was last seen by the witness, and whether the appropriate instructions were given to the witness.21
[CHARGE (a) and (b) IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THE POLICE CONDUCT AN IDENTIFICATION LINEUP PROCEDURE]22
In determining the reliability of the identification, you should also consider whether the identification procedure was properly conducted.
page68image11696 page68image11856
(a) Double-blind: A lineup administrator who knows which lineup member is the suspect may intentionally or unintentionally convey this knowledge to the witness, thereby increasing the chance that the witness will identify the suspect even if the suspect is innocent. For this reason, whenever feasible, lineups and photo-spreads should be conducted by an officer who does not know the identity of the suspect.23
[CHARGE IF BLIND ADMINISTRATOR IS NOT USED]
If a police officer who does not know the suspects identity is not available, then the officer should use a method that does not allow him/her to see the photos as the witness looks at them. In this case, it is alleged that the person who presented the lineup [did/did not] know the identity of the suspect.
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 259-61.
page68image19224
21 22
and address various system and estimator variables.)
23
page68image20728 page68image20888 page68image21048
To help jurors weigh that evidence, they must be told about relevant factors and their effect on reliability. Id. at 219 (asking the Criminal Practice Committee and the Committee on Model Criminal Jury Charges to draft proposed revisions to this charge
page68image23464
Id. at 248-50.
page68image24152
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 8 of 10
[In this case, it is alleged that a police officer who did not know the suspects identity was not available, and the police did/did not compensate for that by conducting a procedure where the officer did not see the photos as the witness looked at them.]
[RESUME MAIN CHARGE]
You may consider this factor when you consider the circumstances under which the identification was made, and when you evaluate the overall reliability of the identification.24
(b) Instructions: You should consider what was or what was not said to the witness prior to viewing a photo array.25 Identification procedures should begin with instructions to the witness that the perpetrator may or may not be in the array and that the witness should not feel compelled to make an identification. The failure to give this instruction can increase the risk of misidentification. If you find that the police [did/did not] give this instruction to the witness, you may take this factor into account when evaluating the identification evidence.26
[CHARGE IF FEEDBACK IS AN ISSUE IN THE CASE]
(c) Feedback: Feedback occurs when police or other witnesses to an event signal to eyewitnesses that they correctly identified the suspect. That confirmation poses a risk of endangering a false sense of confidence in a witness. Feedback can also present a risk of falsely enhancing a witnesss recollection of the quality of his or her view of an event. It is for you to determine whether or not the recollection of the witness was affected by feedback or whether the recollection instead reflects the accurate perceptions of the witness during the event.27
[RESUME MAIN CHARGE]
You may consider whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions, or identifications given by other witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any
page69image17128 page69image17288 page69image17448
24 25 26 27
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 248-50.
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995).
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 250.
Id. at 253-55; see also Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at

page69image20632 page69image20792 page69image20952 page69image21112 page69image21272 page69image21432 page69image21592 page69image21752 page69image21912 page69image22072 page69image22232 page69image22392 page69image22552 page69image22712 page69image22872
781 n.2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n.8 (Utah 1986)).
page69image23960 page69image24120
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 9 of 10
other information or influence, that may have affected the independence of his/her identification.28 Such information can affect the independent nature and reliability of a witnesss identification and inflate witness confidence in that identification.
You are also free to consider any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case that you consider relevant to your determination whether the identifications were reliable. Keep in mind that the presence of any single factor or combination of factor(s), however, is not an indication that a particular witness is incorrect. Instead, you may consider the factor or factor(s) which I have discussed as you assess all of the circumstances of the case, including all of the testimony and documentary evidence in determining whether a particular identification made by a witness is accurate and thus worthy of your consideration as you determine whether the State has met its burden to prove identification beyond a reasonable doubt. Unless the in- court and out-of-court identifications resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the in-court and/or out-of-court identification procedures, they should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of an identification is for you to decide.
If, after consideration of all of the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after consideration of all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt
page70image14752 page70image14912 page70image15072
28
State v. Chen, 208 N.J. 307 (2011).
page70image16272 page70image16432
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 10 of 10
that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
page71image2720 page71image2880
Page 1 of 5 Revised 11/18/11
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
(Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find defendant guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this person is the person who committed the crime. (Defendant) has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proved each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proved beyond a reasonable doubt that (this defendant) is the person who committed it.
The State has presented testimony of [insert name of witness who identified defendant]. You will recall that this witness identified the defendant as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the identification of (defendant) is reliable and believable, or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude that (this defendant) is the person who committed the offense[s] charged.
1
page72image14864 page72image15024 page72image15184 page72image15344 page72image15504
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v. Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981); State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
page72image17560 page72image17720 page72image17880 page72image18040 page72image18200 page72image18360 page72image18520 page72image18680
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 2 of 5

Eyewitness identification evidence must be scrutinized carefully. Human beings do have the ability to recognize other people from past experiences and to identify them at a later time. But human memory is not foolproof. Research has shown that human memory is not at all like a video recording that a witness need only replay to remember what happened. Human memory is far more complex.2
You should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.3
In deciding what weight, if any, to give to the identification testimony, you should consider the following factors [cite appropriate factors]:
page73image9752 page73image9912
(1)
The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention: In evaluating the reliability of the identification, you should assess the witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense and the witnesss degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense. In making this assessment you should consider the following [choose appropriate factors from (a) through (i) below]:
(a) Stress: Even under the best viewing conditions, high levels of stress can reduce an eyewitnesss ability to recall and make an accurate identification. Therefore, you should consider the level of the witnesss stress and whether that stress, if any, distracted the witness or made it harder for him or her to identify the perpetrator.4
State v. Henderson, 208 N.J. 208, 245 (2011). State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007). Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 261-62.
page73image18208
2 3 4
page73image19104 page73image19264 page73image19424 page73image19584 page73image19744 page73image19904 page73image20064
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 3 of 5

page74image1440 page74image1600
5 6 7 8 9
Id. at 265. Although Henderson only addressed alcohol, if there is evidence of
(b) Weapons Focus: You should consider whether the witness saw the weapon during the incident. The presence of a weapon can distract the witness and take the witnesss attention away from the perpetrators face. As a result, the presence of a visible weapon may reduce the reliability of a subsequent identification if the crime is of a short duration. In considering this factor, you should take into account the duration of the crime because the longer the duration of the event, the more time the witness may have to adapt to the presence of the weapon and focus on other details.5
(c) Duration: The amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. Although there is no minimum time required to make an accurate identification a brief or fleeting contact is less likely to produce an accurate identification than a more prolonged exposure to the perpetrator. In addition, the time estimate given by a witness may not always be accurate because witnesses tend to think events lasted longer than they actually did.6
(d) Distance: A person is easier to recognize when close by. The greater the distance between an eyewitness and a perpetrator, the higher the risk of a mistaken identification. In addition, a witnesss estimate of how far he or she was from the perpetrator may not always be accurate because people tend to have difficulty estimating distances.7
(e) Lighting: Inadequate lighting can reduce the reliability of an identification. You should consider the lighting conditions that were present at the time of the alleged crime in this case.8
(f) Intoxication: An identification given by a witness with a high level of alcohol at the time of the incident tends to be more unreliable than an identification given by a witness who did not have a high level of alcohol. 9
(g) Disguises/Changed Appearance: The perpetrators use of a disguise can affect a witnesss ability to both remember and identify the perpetrator. Disguises can be as simple as hats or sunglasses, as well as masks, and can
reduce the accuracy of an identification.10
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 262-63. Id. at 264.
Ibid.
Ibid.

Similarly, if facial features are
page74image23424 page74image23584 page74image23744 page74image23904 page74image24064 page74image24224 page74image24384 page74image24544 page74image24704
impairment by drugs or other substances the parties may want to have the jury consider
this issue.
10
Id. at 266.
page74image26952
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 4 of 5

altered between the time of the event and a subsequent identification procedure, the accuracy of the identification may decrease.11
(2) Prior Description of Perpetrator: Another factor for your consideration is the accuracy of any description the witness gave before identifying the perpetrator. Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided details or was just general in nature, and whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator.
(3) Time Elapsed: Memories fade with time. As a result, delays between the commission of a crime and the time an identification is made can affect the reliability of the identification. In other words, the more time that passes, the greater the possibility that a witnesss memory of a perpetrator will weaken.12
(4) Cross-Racial Effects: The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.13
(5) Catch-all Factor: You may also consider the age of the identifying witness and any other relevant characteristics of the witness.
You may consider whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions, or identifications given by other witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other information or influence, that may have affected the independence of his/her identification.14 Such information can affect the independent nature and reliability of a witnesss identification and inflate witness confidence in that identification.
You are also free to consider any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case that you consider relevant to your determination whether the
page75image18280 page75image18440 page75image18600
11 12 13
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 266.
Id. at 267.
This instruction must be given whenever there is a cross-racial identification. Id.

page75image20856 page75image21016 page75image21176 page75image21336 page75image21496
at 299 (modifying State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 132 (1999)).
14
State v. Chen, 208 N.J. 307 (2011).
page75image23544 page75image23704 page75image23864 page75image24024
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 5 of 5

identification was reliable. Keep in mind that the presence of any single factor or combination of factor(s), however, is not an indication that a particular witness is incorrect. Instead, you may consider the factor or factor(s) which I have discussed as you assess all of the circumstances of the case, including all of the testimony and documentary evidence in determining whether a particular identification made by a witness is accurate and thus worthy of your consideration as you determine whether the State has met its burden to prove identification beyond a reasonable doubt.
Unless the in-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of the identification is for you to decide.
If, after considering all of the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after considering all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
page76image11984 page76image12144
Page 1 of 8 Revised 1/9/12
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
(Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find (defendant) guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this person is the person who committed the crime. (Defendant) has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proved each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proved beyond a reasonable doubt that (this defendant) is the person who committed it.
The State has presented testimony that on a prior occasion before this trial, [insert name of witness who identified defendant] identified (defendant) as the person who committed [insert the offenses charged]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the identification of (defendant) is reliable and believable or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence that (this defendant) is the person who committed the offense[s] charged.
1
page77image14704 page77image14864 page77image15024 page77image15184 page77image15344
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v. Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981); State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
page77image17400 page77image17560 page77image17720 page77image17880 page77image18040 page77image18200 page77image18360 page77image18520
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 2 of 8

Eyewitness identification evidence must be scrutinized carefully. Human beings do have the ability to recognize other people from past experiences and to identify them at a later time. But human memory is not foolproof. Research has shown that human memory is not at all like a video recording that a witness need only replay to remember what happened. Human memory is far more complex.2
You should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of thereliabilityoftheidentification.3 In deciding what weight, if any, to give to the identification testimony, you should consider the following factors [cite appropriate factors]:
(1) The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention: In evaluating the reliability of the identification, you should assess the witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense and the witnesss degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense. In making this assessment you should consider the following [choose appropriate factors from (a) through (i) below]:
page78image12928 page78image13088 page78image13248
2 3 4
(a) Stress: Even under the best viewing conditions, high levels of stress can reduce an eyewitnesss ability to recall and make an accurate identification. Therefore, you should consider the level of the witnesss stress and whether that stress, if any, distracted the witness or made it harder for him or her to identify the perpetrator.4
State v. Henderson, 208 N.J. 208, 245 (2011). State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007). Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 261-62.
page78image18376 page78image18536 page78image18696 page78image18856 page78image19016 page78image19176 page78image19336
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 3 of 8

page79image1440 page79image1600
5 6 7 8 9
Id. at 265. Although Henderson only addressed alcohol, if there is evidence of
(b) Weapons Focus: You should consider whether the witness saw the weapon during the incident. The presence of a weapon can distract the witness and take the witnesss attention away from the perpetrators face. As a result, the presence of a visible weapon may reduce the reliability of a subsequent identification if the crime is of a short duration. In considering this factor, you should take into account the duration of the crime because the longer the duration of the event, the more time the witness may have to adapt to the presence of the weapon and focus on other details.5
(c) Duration: The amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. Although there is no minimum time required to make an accurate identification a brief or fleeting contact is less likely to produce an accurate identification than a more prolonged exposure to the perpetrator. In addition, the time estimate given by a witness may not always be accurate because witnesses tend to think events lasted longer than they actually did.6
(d) Distance: A person is easier to recognize when close by. The greater the distance between an eyewitness and a perpetrator, the higher the risk of a mistaken identification. In addition, a witnesss estimate of how far he or she was from the perpetrator may not always be accurate because people tend to have difficulty estimating distances.7
(e) Lighting: Inadequate lighting can reduce the reliability of an identification. You should consider the lighting conditions that were present at the time of the alleged crime in this case.8
(f) Intoxication: An identification given by a witness with a high level of alcohol at the time of the incident tends to be more unreliable than an identification given by a witness who did not have a high level of alcohol. 9
(g) Disguises/Changed Appearance: The perpetrators use of a disguise can affect a witnesss ability to both remember and identify the perpetrator. Disguises can be as simple as hats or sunglasses, as well as masks, and can
reduce the accuracy of an identification.10
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 262-63. Id. at 264.
Ibid.
Ibid.

Similarly, if facial features are
page79image23424 page79image23584 page79image23744 page79image23904 page79image24064 page79image24224 page79image24384 page79image24544 page79image24704
impairment by drugs or other substances the parties may want to have the jury consider
this issue.
10
Id. at 266.
page79image26952
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 4 of 8

altered between the time of the event and a subsequent identification procedure, the accuracy of the identification may decrease.11
(2) Prior Description of Perpetrator: Another factor for your consideration is the accuracy of any description the witness gave before identifying the perpetrator. Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided details or was just general in nature, and whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator.
(3) Confidence and Accuracy: You heard testimony that (insert name of witness) made a statement at the time of his/her identification of defendant from the photo array/line-up concerning his/her level of certainty that the photograph he/she selected is in fact the person who committed the crime. Although some research has found that highly confident witnesses are more likely to make accurate identifications, eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy.12
(4) Time Elapsed: Memories fade with time. As a result, delays between the commission of a crime and the time an identification is made can affect the reliability of the identification. In other words, the more time that passes, the greater the possibility that a witnesss memory of a perpetrator will weaken.13
(5) Cross-Racial Effects: The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.14
(6) Catch-all Factor: You may also consider the age of the identifying witness and any other relevant characteristics of the witness.
In evaluating the reliability of a witnesss identification, you should also consider the circumstances under which the out-of-court identification was made, and whether or not it was the result of a suggestive procedure, including everything done or said by law
page80image19536 page80image19696 page80image19856
11 12 13 14
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 266.
Id. at 253-55.
Id. at 267.
This instruction must be given whenever there is a cross-racial identification. Id.

page80image22480 page80image22640 page80image22800 page80image22960 page80image23120 page80image23280
at 299 (modifying State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 132 (1999)).
page80image24288 page80image24448
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 5 of 8

enforcement to the witness during the identification process. You should consider the following factors: [Charge if appropriate]:15
(1) Lineup Composition: A suspect should not stand out from other members of the lineup. The reason is simple: an array of look-alikes forces witnesses to examine their memory. In addition, a biased lineup may inflate a witnesss confidence in the identification because the selection process seemed so easy to the witness.16 It is, of course, for you to determine whether the lineup was biased or not and whether the composition of the lineup had any affect on the reliability of the identification.
(2) Fillers: Lineups should include a number of possible choices for the witness, commonly referred to as fillers. The greater the number of choices, the more likely the procedure will serve as a reliable test of the witnesss memory. A minimum of six persons or photos should be included in the lineup.17
(3) Multiple Viewing: When a witness views a person in multiple identification procedures, the witnesss memory of the actual perpetrator can be replaced by the witnesss memory of the person seen in the multiple procedures. In this way, when a witness views an innocent suspect in multiple identification procedures, the risk of mistaken identification is increased. You may consider whether the witness viewed the suspect multiple times and, if so, whether viewing the suspect in multiple procedures affected the reliability of the identification. 18
[CHARGE IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THERE IS A SHOWUP PROCEDURE]
(4) Showups: In this case, the witness identified the defendant during a showup, that is, defendant was the only person shown to the witness at that time. Even though such a procedure is suggestive in nature, sometimes it is necessary for the police to conduct a showup, or one-on-one, identification procedure. Although the benefits of a fresh memory may balance the risks of undue suggestion, showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk
page81image17896 page81image18056 page81image18216
15
The followingfactorsconsistofthesystem...variables...forwhich[theCourt]
found scientific support that is generally accepted by experts. Henderson, supra, 208
N.J. at 298-99.
page81image20440 page81image20600
16 17 18
Id. at 251.
Ibid.
Id. at 255-56. If either mugshot exposure (no identification in first lineup/photo

page81image22656 page81image22816 page81image22976 page81image23136
array, later identification of someone in first procedure in second lineup/photo array) or mugshot commitment (selection of person in lineup who was identified in previous photo array) are part of the evidence, the jury should be instructed on the concepts implicated by those terms without using the word mugshot. See model charge on Identity-Police Photos.
page81image26320
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 6 of 8

of misidentification. Also, police officers must instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification. In determining whether the identification is reliable or the result of undue suggestive procedure, you should consider how much time elapsed after the perpetrator was last seen by the witness, and whether the appropriate instructions were given to the witness.19
[CHARGE (a) AND (b) IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THE POLICE CONDUCT AN IDENTIFICATION LINEUP PROCEDURE]20
In determining the reliability of the identification, you should also consider whether the identification procedure was properly conducted.
page82image7256 page82image7416
(a) Double-blind: A lineup administrator who knows which lineup member is the suspect may intentionally or unintentionally convey this knowledge to the witness, thereby increasing the chance that the witness will identify the suspect even if the suspect is innocent. For this reason, whenever feasible, lineups and photo-spreads should be conducted by an officer who does not know the identity of the suspect.21
[CHARGE IF BLIND ADMINISTRATOR IS NOT USED]
If a police officer who does not know the suspects identity is not available, then the officer should use a method that does not allow him/her to see the photos as the witness looks at them. In this case, it is alleged that the person who presented the lineup [did/did not] know the identity of the suspect.
[In this case, it is alleged that a police officer who did not know the suspects identity was not available, and the police did/did not compensate for that by conducting a procedure where the officer did not see the photos as the witness looked at them.]
[RESUME MAIN CHARGE]
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 259-61.
page82image17488
19 20
and address various system and estimator variables.)
21
page82image18992 page82image19152 page82image19312
To help jurors weigh that evidence, they must be told about relevant factors and their effect on reliability. Id. at 219 (asking the Criminal Practice Committee and the Committee on Model Criminal Jury Charges to draft proposed revisions to this charge
page82image21728
Id. at 248-50.
page82image22416
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 7 of 8

You may consider this factor when you consider the circumstances under which the identification was made, and when you evaluate the overall reliability of the identification.22
(b) Instructions: You should consider what was or what was not said to the witness prior to viewing a photo array.23 Identification procedures should begin with instructions to the witness that the perpetrator may or may not be in the array and that the witness should not feel compelled to make an identification. The failure to give this instruction can increase the risk of misidentification. If you find that the police [did/did not] give this instruction to the witness, you may take this factor into account when evaluating the identification evidence.24
[CHARGE IF FEEDBACK IS AN ISSUE IN THE CASE]
(c) Feedback: Feedback occurs when police or other witnesses to an event signal to eyewitnesses that they correctly identified the suspect. That confirmation poses a risk of endangering a false sense of confidence in a witness. Feedback can also present a risk of falsely enhancing a witnesss recollection of the quality of his or her view of an event. It is for you to determine whether or not the recollection of the witness was affected by feedback or whether the recollection instead reflects the accurate perceptions of the witness during the event.25
[RESUME MAIN CHARGE]
You may consider whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions, or identifications given by other witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other information or influence, that may have affected the independence of his/her
page83image14968 page83image15128 page83image15288
22 23 24 25
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 248-50.
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995).
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 250.
Id. at 253-55; see also Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at

page83image18472 page83image18632 page83image18792 page83image18952 page83image19112 page83image19272 page83image19432 page83image19592 page83image19752 page83image19912 page83image20072 page83image20232 page83image20392 page83image20552 page83image20712
781 n.2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n.8 (Utah 1986)).
page83image21800 page83image21960
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 8 of 8

identification.26 Such information can affect the independent nature and reliability of a witnesss identification and inflate witness confidence in that identification.
You are also free to consider any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case that you consider relevant to your determination whether the identification was reliable. Keep in mind that the presence of any single factor or combination of factor(s), however, is not an indication that a particular witness is incorrect. Instead, you may consider the factor or factor(s) which I have discussed as you assess all of the circumstances of the case, including all of the testimony and documentary evidence in determining whether a particular identification made by a witness is accurate and thus worthy of your consideration as you determine whether the State has met its burden to prove identification beyond a reasonable doubt.
Unless the out-of-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the out-of-court identification procedure, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of the identification is for you to decide.
If, after considering all of the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after consideration of all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
page84image15248 page84image15408 page84image15568
26
State v. Chen, 208 N.J. 307 (2011).
page84image16768 page84image16928
APPENDIX B CURRENT CHARGES
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS
(Defendant) as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find this defendant guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed the crime. The defendant has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proved each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proved beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed it.
The State has presented the testimony of [insert name of witness who identified defendant]. You will recall that this witness identified the defendant in court as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. The State also presented testimony that on a prior occasion before this trial, this witness identified the defendant as the person who committed this offense [these offenses]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the witness identification of the defendant is reliable and believable, or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v. Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-
Revised 6/4/07
page86image16432 page86image16592 page86image16752 page86image16912 page86image17072
1
293 (1981); State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
page86image18304 page86image18464 page86image18624 page86image18784 page86image18944 page86image19104 page86image19264
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 2 of 6
to conclude that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. You should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.2
In evaluating the identifications, you should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identifications were based, and the witness ability to make those observations and perceptions. If you determine that the out-of-court identification is not reliable, you may still consider the witness in-court identification of the defendant if you find it to be reliable. Unless the in-court identification resulted from the witness observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the out-of-court identification procedure, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issues of the trustworthiness of both the in-court and out-of-court identifications are for you to decide.3
To decide whether the identification testimony is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged, you should evaluate the testimony of the witness in light of the factors for considering credibility that I have already explained to you. In addition, you may consider the following factors [cite appropriate factors]:4
page87image14624 page87image14784 page87image14944
2 3
of-court identification, and jurys credibility determinations).
4
State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007).
page87image17136 page87image17296
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 229-232, 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1933-1935, 1940 (manner in which lineup or other identification procedure conducted relevant to reliability of out-of-court identification and in-court identification following out-
page87image19328 page87image19488 page87image19648
The first five factors listed below were enumerated in Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. 188, 199, 93 S.Ct. 375, 382 (1972), and United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940, as the factors to be considered in evaluating the likelihood of misidentification. New Jersey courts employ the same analysis. State v. Madison, 109 N.J. 223, 239-240 (1988). See also State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503, 520 (App. Div. 1995).
page87image23488 page87image23648 page87image23808 page87image23968 page87image24128 page87image24288 page87image24448 page87image24608 page87image24768 page87image24928 page87image25088 page87image25248 page87image25408
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 3 of 6
[If necessary or appropriate for purposes of clarity, the judge may comment on any evidence relevant to any of the following factors]5
page88image3224 page88image3384 page88image3544
5
(1) The witness opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense.6
(2) The witness degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense.7
(3) The accuracy of any description the witness gave prior to identifying the perpetrator.8 (4) The degree of certainty expressed by the witness in making any identification.9
See State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 128 (1999) (when identification is a critical issue in the case, the trial court is
page88image8488 page88image8648 page88image8808
obligated to give the jury discrete and specific instruction that provides appropriate guidelines to focus the jurys attention on how
to analyze and consider the trustworthiness of eyewitness identification); State v. Green, 86 N.J. at 292, 293 (noting that model
charge could have been used as a guide, court holds that the defendant had a right to expect that the appropriate guidelines
would be given, focusing the jurys attention on how to analyze and consider the factual issues with regard to the trustworthiness
of [the witnesss] in-court identification); but see State v. Robinson, 165 N.J. 32, 42-45 (2000) (reaffirming obligation under
Green to explain abstract identification factors in factual context of case, but holding that court need not necessarily summarize
weaknesses of States evidence); see generally, State v. Gartland, 149 N.J. 456, 475 (1997) (holding that jury charges must relate
the law to the specific facts in a case); State v. A. Gross, 121 N.J. 1 (1990) (same); State v. Concepcion, 111 N.J. 373 (1988)
(same). 6
be appropriate to add the following to factor (1): . . . including the nature of the event being observed and the likelihood that the
witness would perceive, remember, and relate it correctly. State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (2006) (quoting State v. Ramirez,
817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)).
page88image20576 page88image20736 page88image20896 page88image21056 page88image21216 page88image21376 page88image21536 page88image21696 page88image21856 page88image22016 page88image22176 page88image22336 page88image22496 page88image22656 page88image22816
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include the witnesss ability to observe what he/she said he/she saw, the amount of time during which the witness saw the perpetrator, the distance from which the witness saw the perpetrator, and the lighting conditions at the time. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. 98, 114, 97 S.Ct. 2243, 2253 (1977); Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200-201, 93 S.Ct. at 382; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 239.
Where supported by evidence that the victim might have difficulty perceiving, recalling, or relating the events, it may
page88image27712 page88image27872 page88image28032 page88image28192 page88image28352 page88image28512 page88image28672 page88image28832 page88image28992 page88image29152 page88image29312 page88image29472
7
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether the witness was merely a passing or casual observer or one
who would be expected to pay scrupulous attention to detail, whether the witness was involved in a direct confrontation with the
perpetrator, whether the witness was nervous, shocked or scared as a result of any confrontation with the perpetrator, and whether
the witnesss attention was focused on or away from the perpetrators features. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97
S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93 S.Ct. at 382-383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240.
page88image35608 page88image35768 page88image35928 page88image36088 page88image36248 page88image36408 page88image36568 page88image36728 page88image36888
8
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after
observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided
details or was just general in nature, whether the witness testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior
description of the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v.Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93
S.Ct. at 383; United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240-241; State v. Edmonds,
293 N.J. Super. 113 (App. Div. 1996).
9
Other relevant facts include whether, at a time prior to making the identification of this defendant, the witness either failed to identify the defendant or identified another person as the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at 383; Foster v. California, 394 U.S. 440, 442-443 & n.2, 89 S.Ct. 1127,
page88image46880 page88image47040 page88image47200 page88image47360 page88image47520 page88image47680 page88image47840 page88image48000 page88image48160 page88image48320 page88image48480 page88image48640 page88image48800 page88image48960
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether witnesses making the identification received inadvertent or intentional confirmation, whether certainty was expressed at the time of the identification or some time later, whether intervening events following the identification affected the witnesss certainty, and whether the identification was made spontaneously and remained consistent thereafter. See N.J. Attorney General Guidelines for Preparing and Conducting Photo and Live Lineup Identification Procedures, April 18, 2001, at 2 (quoted in Herrera, 187 N.J. at 190); National Institute of Justice, Convicted by Juries, Exonerated by Science, June 1996, at 24 (available at https://www.ndjrs.gov/pdffiles/dnaevid.pdf); Gary Wells & Amy Bradfield, Good, You Identified the Suspect, 83 J. Applied Psychol. 360 (1998); Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781. Whether the witness made an identification quickly upon viewing the suspect, or whether the witness hesitated, may also be a relevant fact. See S. Sporer, Eyewitness Identification Accuracy, Confidence, and Decision Times in Simultaneous and Sequential Lineups, 78 J. Applied Psychol. 22, 23 (1993).
page88image56984 page88image57144 page88image57304 page88image57464 page88image57624 page88image57784 page88image57944 page88image58104 page88image58264 page88image58424 page88image58584 page88image58744 page88image58904 page88image59064 page88image59224 page88image59384 page88image59544 page88image59704 page88image59864 page88image60024 page88image60184
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 4 of 6
(5) The length of time between the witness observation of the offense and the first identification.10
  1. (6) Discrepancies or inconsistencies between identifications, if any.11
  2. (7) The circumstances under which any out-of-court identification was made, and whether or not it was the product of a suggestive procedure12, including everything done or said by law enforcement to the witness before, during, or after the identification process.13 In making this determination you may consider the
    following circumstances:
[REFER TO CIRCUMSTANCES OF THE IDENTIFICATION PROCEDURE AS NECESSARY FOR CLARITY, CHOOSING AS APPROPRIATE ANY OF THE FOLLOWING FACTORS, OR ANY OTHER FACTORS RELATING TO SUGGESTIVENESS, THAT ARE SUPPORTED BY THE EVIDENCE:]
  • whether anything was said to the witness prior to viewing a photo array, line-up or showup;14
  • whether a photo array shown to the witness contained multiple photographs of the defendant;15
  • whether all in the lineup but the [defendant] were known to the identifying witness;16
    1128-1129 & n.2 (1969); United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 241. Madison cautions, with respect to an identification witnesss demonstrated certainty in his testimony, that a witnesss feeling of confidence in the details of memory generally do not validly measure the accuracy of the recollection, and that [i]n fact, witnesses frequently become more confident of the correctness of their memory over time while the actual memory trace is probably decaying. Id. at 241-242 (quoting W.LaFave and J.Israel, Criminal Procedure).
page89image14744 page89image14904 page89image15064 page89image15224 page89image15384 page89image15544 page89image15704 page89image15864 page89image16024 page89image16184 page89image16344
10
383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 242.
See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115-116, 97 S.Ct. at 2253-2254; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether the witness identification at trial was different from, or the
page89image19568 page89image19728 page89image19888 page89image20048 page89image20208 page89image20368 page89image20528 page89image20688 page89image20848
11
same as, any prior identification that took place out-of-court. See State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. at 118.
page89image22280 page89image22440 page89image22600
12
Refer to the New Jersey Attorney General Guidelines, footnote 8 supra. The court should focus on any allegations of
page89image24280 page89image24440
suggestive words or conduct by law enforcement or other persons that may effect the suggestiveness of the identification
procedures.
13
See State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493 (2006), in which the New Jersey Supreme Court addressed the propriety of a show-
page89image27584 page89image27744 page89image27904
up identification; the majority opinion concluded that, while such a procedure is inherently suggestive, the identification
procedure employed there was reliable and did not result in a substantial likelihood of misidentification.
14 15 16
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995). Id.
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 233, 87 S.Ct. at 1935.

page89image32144 page89image32304 page89image32464 page89image32624 page89image32784 page89image32944 page89image33104
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 5 of 6
  • whether the other participants in a lineup were grossly dissimilar in appearance to the [defendant];17
  • whether only the [defendant] was required to wear distinctive clothing which the culprit allegedly wore;18
  • whether the witness is told by the police that they have caught the culprit after which the defendant is brought before the witness alone or is viewed in jail;19
  • whether the [defendant] is pointed out before or during a lineup;20
  • whether the witnesss identification was made spontaneously and remained consistent
    thereafter;21
  • whether the individual conducting the lineup either indicated to the witness that a suspect
    was present or failed to warn the witness that the perpetrator may or may not be in the
    procedure;22
  • whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions, or identifications given by
page90image9496 page90image9656 page90image9816
17 18 19 20 21 22
other witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other information or influence that may have affected the independence of his/her identification.23
[CHARGE IN ALL CASES:]
(8) Any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case which you consider relevant to your determination whether the identifications were reliable.
[(9) Jury should be charged on any other relevant factor present in the case24]
Id.
Id.
Id.
Id., 87 S.Ct. at 1935-1936.
See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting State v. Ramirez, 817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)).
See N.J. Attorney General Guidelines, supra, Guideline I.B. (requiring administrator to instruct witness that perpetrator

page90image17328 page90image17488 page90image17648 page90image17808 page90image17968 page90image18128 page90image18288 page90image18448 page90image18608 page90image18768 page90image18928 page90image19088 page90image19248
may not be present); State v. Ledbetter, 881 A.2d 290 (Ct. 2005) (requiring jury instruction to that effect).
23
24
page90image21048 page90image21208
See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781 n. 2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n. 8 (Utah 1986)).
page90image22920 page90image23080 page90image23240 page90image23400 page90image23560 page90image23720 page90image23880 page90image24040
The list of factors enumerated in Biggers and Madison is not exhaustive. See State v. White, 158 N.J. 230, (1999) (in declining to find plain error in identification charge, court notes that instruction went beyond model charge, noting the discrepancy ... between identifications made by different witnesses). Additional relevant factors that should be brought to jurys attention include the witnesss inability to make an in-court identification if asked to do so while on the witness stand, any failure on the part of the State to record a line-up or preserve a photo array, as bearing upon the probative value of the out-of-court identification, see State v. Delgado, 188 N.J. 48, 63 (2006); State v. Earle, 60 N.J. 550, 552 (1972); State v. Peterkin, 226 N.J. Super. 25, 46 (App. Div. 1988), and any discrepancies between identifications made by different witnesses, State v. White, 158 N.J. 230, 248.
page90image31216 page90image31376 page90image31536 page90image31696 page90image31856 page90image32016 page90image32176 page90image32336 page90image32496 page90image32656 page90image32816 page90image32976 page90image33136 page90image33296 page90image33456
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 6 of 6
[IN THE APPROPRIATE CASE,25 CHARGE THE FOLLOWING FACTOR:]
(10) The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.26
[CHARGE IN ALL CASES:]
Unless the in-court and out-of-court identifications resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the in-court and/or out-of-court identification procedures, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of the identification is for you to decide.
If, after consideration of all of the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after consideration of all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
page91image12552 page91image12712 page91image12872
25
An instruction that cross-racial identification is a factor to be considered should be given only when ... identification is
a critical issue in the case, and an eyewitnesss cross-racial identification is not corroborated by other evidence giving it
independent reliability. State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. at 132; see also State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007)
26
page91image16912 page91image17072 page91image17232 page91image17392 page91image17552 page91image17712
Cromedy holds that in order for the jury to determine the reliability of a cross-racial identification not corroborated by independent evidence, the jury must be informed of the potential risks associated with such identifications, that the jury must be instructed about the possible significance of the cross-racial identification factor.... 158 N.J. at 132-133. In State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007), the New Jersey Supreme Court refused to rule that cross-ethnic charges were required in cases involving an individuals identification of a person of another ethnic background.
page91image22176 page91image22336 page91image22496 page91image22656 page91image22816
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v. Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981);
Revised 6/4/07
page92image2792
(Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find (defendant) guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this person is the person who committed the crime. (Defendant) has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proved each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proved beyond a reasonable doubt that (this defendant) is the person who committed it.
The State has presented testimony of [insert name of witness who identified defendant]. You will recall that this witness identified the defendant as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the identification of (defendant) is reliable and believable or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude that (this defendant) is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. You should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence,
page92image19000 page92image19160 page92image19320
1
State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
page92image20472 page92image20632 page92image20792 page92image20952 page92image21112 page92image21272 page92image21432
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 2 of 5

standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.2 In deciding what weight, if any, to give to the identification testimony, you may consider the following factors [cite appropriate factors]:3
[If necessary or appropriate for purposes of clarity, the judge may comment on any evidence relevant to any of the following factors]4
page93image4936 page93image5096 page93image5256
2 3
(1) The witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense.5
(2) The witnesss degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense.6
(3) The accuracy of any description the witness gave prior to identifying the perpetrator.7 (4) The degree of certainty expressed by the witness in making the identification.8
State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007).
The first five factors listed below were enumerated in Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. 188, 199, 93 S.Ct. 375, 382 (1972), and
page93image10880 page93image11040 page93image11200 page93image11360 page93image11520
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940, as the factors to be considered in evaluating the likelihood of
misidentification. New Jersey courts employ the same analysis. State v. Madison, 109 N.J. 223, 239-240 (1988). See also State
v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503, 520 (App. Div. 1995).
page93image14600 page93image14760 page93image14920 page93image15080 page93image15240 page93image15400 page93image15560 page93image15720
4
See State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 128 (1999) (when identification is a critical issue in the case, the trial court is
page93image17520 page93image17680 page93image17840 page93image18000 page93image18160
obligated to give the jury discrete and specific instruction that provides appropriate guidelines to focus the jurys attention on how
to analyze and consider the trustworthiness of eyewitness identification); State v. Green, 86 N.J. at 292, 293 (noting that model
charge could have been used as a guide, court holds that the defendant had a right to expect that the appropriate guidelines
would be given, focusing the jurys attention on how to analyze and consider the factual issues with regard to the trustworthiness
of [the witnesss] in-court identification); but see State v. Robinson, 165 N.J. 32, 42-45 (2000) (reaffirming obligation under
Green to explain abstract identification factors in factual context of case, but holding that court need not necessarily summarize
weaknesses of States evidence); see generally, State v. Gartland, 149 N.J. 456, 475 (1997) (holding that jury charges must relate
the law to the specific facts in a case); State v. A. Gross, 121 N.J. 1 (1990) (same); State v. Concepcion, 111 N.J. 373 (1988)
(same). 5
be appropriate to add the following to factor (1): . . . including the nature of the event being observed and the likelihood that the
witness would perceive, remember, and relate it correctly. State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (2006) (quoting State v. Ramirez,
817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)).
page93image29928 page93image30088 page93image30248 page93image30408 page93image30568 page93image30728 page93image30888 page93image31048 page93image31208 page93image31368 page93image31528 page93image31688 page93image31848 page93image32008 page93image32168
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include the witnesss ability to observe what he/she said he/she saw, the amount of time during which the witness saw the perpetrator, the distance from which the witness saw the perpetrator, and the lighting conditions at the time. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. 98, 114, 97 S.Ct. 2243, 2253 (1977); Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200-201, 93 S.Ct. at 382; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 239.
Where supported by evidence that the victim might have difficulty perceiving, recalling, or relating the events, it may
page93image37064 page93image37224 page93image37384 page93image37544 page93image37704 page93image37864 page93image38024 page93image38184 page93image38344 page93image38504 page93image38664 page93image38824
6
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether the witness was merely a passing or casual observer or one
who would be expected to pay scrupulous attention to detail, whether the witness was involved in a direct confrontation with the
perpetrator, whether the witness was nervous, shocked or scared as a result of any confrontation with the perpetrator, and whether
the witnesss attention was focused on or away from the perpetrators features. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97
S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93 S.Ct. at 382-383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240.
page93image44960 page93image45120 page93image45280 page93image45440 page93image45600 page93image45760 page93image45920 page93image46080 page93image46240
7
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after
observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided
details or was just general in nature, whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior
description of the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v.Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93
S.Ct. at 383; United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240-241; State v. Edmonds,
293 N.J. Super. 113 (App. Div. 1996).
8
page93image53168 page93image53328 page93image53488 page93image53648 page93image53808 page93image53968 page93image54128 page93image54288 page93image54448 page93image54608 page93image54768 page93image54928 page93image55088 page93image55248
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether witnesses making the identification received inadvertent or intentional confirmation, whether certainty was expressed at the time of the identification or some time later, whether intervening events following the identification affected the witnesss certainty, and whether the identification was made spontaneously and
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 3 of 5

(5) The length of time between the witnesss observation of the perpetrator during the offense and the identification.9
(6) The circumstances under which the identification was made, and whether or not it was the product of a suggestive procedure10, including everything done or said by law enforcement to the witness before, during, or after the identification process.11 In making this determination you may consider the following circumstances:
[REFER TO CIRCUMSTANCES OF THE IDENTIFICATION PROCEDURE AS NECESSARY FOR CLARITY, CHOOSING AS APPROPRIATE ANY OF THE FOLLOWING FACTORS, OR ANY OTHER FACTORS RELATING TO SUGGESTIVENESS, THAT ARE SUPPORTED BY THE EVIDENCE:]
whether anything was said to the witness prior to viewing a photo array, line-up or showup;12
whether a photo array shown to the witness contained multiple photographs of the defendant;13
remained consistent thereafter. See N.J. Attorney General Guidelines for Preparing and Conducting Photo and Live Lineup Identification Procedures, April 18, 2001, at 2 (quoted in Herrera, 187 N.J. at 190); National Institute of Justice, Convicted by Juries, Exonerated by Science, June 1996, at 24 (available at https://www.ndjrs.gov/pdffiles/dnaevid.pdf); Gary Wells & Amy Bradfield, Good, You Identified the Suspect, 83 J. Applied Psychol. 360 (1998); Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781. Whether the witness made an identification quickly upon viewing the suspect, or whether the witness hesitated, may also be a relevant fact. See S. Sporer, Eyewitness Identification Accuracy, Confidence, and Decision Times in Simultaneous and Sequential Lineups, 78 J. Applied Psychol. 22, 23 (1993).
Other relevant facts include whether, at a time prior to making the identification of this defendant, the witness either failed to identify the defendant or identified another person as the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at 383; Foster v. California, 394 U.S. 440, 442-443 & n.2, 89 S.Ct. 1127, 1128-1129 & n.2 (1969); United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 241. Madison cautions, with respect to an identification witnesss demonstrated certainty in his testimony, that a witnesss feeling of confidence in the details of memory generally do not validly measure the accuracy of the recollection, and that [i]n fact, witnesses frequently become more confident of the correctness of their memory over time while the actual memory trace is probably decaying. Id. at 241-242 (quoting W.LaFave and J.Israel, Criminal Procedure).
page94image21672 page94image21832 page94image21992 page94image22152 page94image22312 page94image22472 page94image22632 page94image22792 page94image22952 page94image23112 page94image23272 page94image23432 page94image23592 page94image23752 page94image23912 page94image24072 page94image24232 page94image24392 page94image24552 page94image24712 page94image24872 page94image25032 page94image25192 page94image25352 page94image25512 page94image25672 page94image25832 page94image25992 page94image26152 page94image26312 page94image26472 page94image26632
9
383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 242.
page94image27664
See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115-116, 97 S.Ct. at 2253-2254; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at
page94image29112 page94image29272 page94image29432 page94image29592 page94image29752 page94image29912 page94image30072
10
Refer to the New Jersey Attorney General Guidelines, footnote 8 supra. The court should focus on any allegations of
page94image31752 page94image31912 page94image32072 page94image32232
suggestive words or conduct by law enforcement or other persons that may effect the suggestiveness of the identification
procedures.
11
See State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493 (2006), in which the New Jersey Supreme Court addressed the propriety of a show-
page94image35376 page94image35536 page94image35696
up identification; the majority opinion concluded that, while such a procedure is inherently suggestive, the identification
procedure employed there was reliable and did not result in a substantial likelihood of misidentification.
12 13
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995). Id.
page94image39208 page94image39368 page94image39528 page94image39688
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 4 of 5

  • whether all in the lineup but the [defendant] were known to the identifying witness;14
  • whether the other participants in a lineup were grossly dissimilar in appearance to the
    [defendant];15
  • whether only the [defendant] was required to wear distinctive clothing which the culprit
    allegedly wore;16
  • whether the witness is told by the police that they have caught the culprit after which the
    defendant is brought before the witness alone or is viewed in jail;17
  • whether the [defendant] is pointed out before or during a lineup;18
  • whether the witnesss identification was made spontaneously and remained consistent
    thereafter;19
  • whether the individual conducting the lineup either indicated to the witness that a suspect
    was present or failed to warn the witness that the perpetrator may or may not be in the
    procedure;20
  • whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions, or identifications given by
page95image10624 page95image10784 page95image10944
14 15 16 17 18 19 20
other witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other information or influence that may have affected the independence of his/her identification.21
[CHARGE IN ALL CASES:]
(7) Any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case which you consider relevant to your determination of whether the out-of-court identification was reliable.
[(8) Jury should be charged on any other relevant factor present in the case22]
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 233, 87 S.Ct. at 1935. Id.
Id.
Id.

Id., 87 S.Ct. at 1935-1936.
See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting State v. Ramirez, 817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)).
See N.J. Attorney General Guidelines, supra, Guideline I.B. (requiring administrator to instruct witness that perpetrator

page95image19480 page95image19640 page95image19800 page95image19960 page95image20120 page95image20280 page95image20440 page95image20600 page95image20760 page95image20920 page95image21080 page95image21240 page95image21400 page95image21560 page95image21720 page95image21880
may not be present); State v. Ledbetter, 881 A.2d 290 (Ct. 2005) (requiring jury instruction to that effect).
21
22
page95image23680 page95image23840
See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781 n. 2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n. 8 (Utah 1986)).
page95image25552 page95image25712 page95image25872 page95image26032 page95image26192 page95image26352 page95image26512 page95image26672
The list of factors enumerated in Biggers and Madison is not exhaustive. See State v. White, 158 N.J. 230, (1999) (in declining to find plain error in identification charge, court notes that instruction went beyond model charge, noting the discrepancy ... between identifications made by different witnesses). Additional relevant factors that should be brought to jurys attention include the witnesss inability to make an in-court identification if asked to do so while on the witness stand, any failure on the part of the State to record a line-up or preserve a photo array, as bearing upon the probative value of the out-of-court identification, see State v. Delgado, 188 N.J. 48, 63 (2006); State v. Earle, 60 N.J. 550, 552 (1972); State v. Peterkin, 226 N.J. Super. 25, 46 (App. Div. 1988), and any discrepancies between identifications made by different witnesses, State v. White, 158
page95image33624 page95image33784 page95image33944 page95image34104 page95image34264 page95image34424 page95image34584 page95image34744 page95image34904 page95image35064 page95image35224 page95image35384 page95image35544 page95image35704
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 5 of 5

[IN THE APPROPRIATE CASE,23 CHARGE THE FOLLOWING FACTOR:]
(9) The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.24
[CHARGE IN ALL CASES:]
Unless the in-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of the identification is for you to decide.
If, after considering all the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after considering all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
page96image11472 page96image11632 page96image11792
N.J. 230, 248.
page96image12480
23
An instruction that cross-racial identification is a factor to be considered should be given only when ... identification is
a critical issue in the case, and an eyewitnesss cross-racial identification is not corroborated by other evidence giving it
independent reliability. State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. at 132; see also State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007).
24
page96image16520 page96image16680 page96image16840 page96image17000 page96image17160 page96image17320
Cromedy holds that in order for the jury to determine the reliability of a cross-racial identification not corroborated by independent evidence, the jury must be informed of the potential risks associated with such identifications, that the jury must be instructed about the possible significance of the cross-racial identification factor.... 158 N.J. at 132-33. In State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007), the New Jersey Supreme Court refused to rule that cross-ethnic charges were required in cases involving an individuals identification of a person of another ethnic background.
page96image21784 page96image21944 page96image22104 page96image22264
Revised 6/4/07 IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
(Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find (defendant) guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this person is the person who committed the crime. (Defendant) has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proved each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proved beyond a reasonable doubt that (this defendant) is the person who committed it.
The State has presented testimony that on a prior occasion before this trial, [insert name of witness who identified defendant] identified (defendant) as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the identification of (defendant) is reliable and believable or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude that (this defendant) is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. You should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence,
page97image17096 page97image17256 page97image17416 page97image17576 page97image17736 page97image17896
1
State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v. Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981);
page97image20296 page97image20456 page97image20616 page97image20776 page97image20936 page97image21096 page97image21256
IDENTIFICATION:OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 2 of 5

standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.2 In deciding what weight, if any, to give to the identification testimony, you may consider the following factors [cite appropriate factors]:3
[If necessary or appropriate for purposes of clarity, the judge may comment on any evidence relevant to any of the following factors]4
page98image4912 page98image5072 page98image5232
2
3
(1) The witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense.5
(2) The witnesss degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense.6
(3) The accuracy of any description the witness gave prior to identifying the perpetrator.7 (4) The degree of certainty expressed by the witness in making the identification.8
State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007).
The first five factors listed below were enumerated in Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. 188, 199, 93 S.Ct. 375, 382 (1972), and
page98image10968 page98image11128 page98image11288 page98image11448 page98image11608
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940, as the factors to be considered in evaluating the likelihood of
misidentification. New Jersey courts employ the same analysis. State v. Madison, 109 N.J. 223, 239-240 (1988). See also State
v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503, 520 (App. Div. 1995).
page98image14688 page98image14848 page98image15008 page98image15168 page98image15328 page98image15488 page98image15648 page98image15808
4
See State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 128 (1999) (when identification is a critical issue in the case, the trial court is
page98image17608 page98image17768 page98image17928 page98image18088 page98image18248
obligated to give the jury discrete and specific instruction that provides appropriate guidelines to focus the jurys attention on how
to analyze and consider the trustworthiness of eyewitness identification); State v. Green, 86 N.J. at 292, 293 (noting that model
charge could have been used as a guide, court holds that the defendant had a right to expect that the appropriate guidelines
would be given, focusing the jurys attention on how to analyze and consider the factual issues with regard to the trustworthiness
of [the witnesss] in-court identification); but see State v. Robinson, 165 N.J. 32, 42-45 (2000) (reaffirming obligation under
Green to explain abstract identification factors in factual context of case, but holding that court need not necessarily summarize
weaknesses of States evidence); see generally, State v. Gartland, 149 N.J. 456, 475 (1997) (holding that jury charges must relate
the law to the specific facts in a case); State v. A. Gross, 121 N.J. 1 (1990) (same); State v. Concepcion, 111 N.J. 373 (1988)
(same). 5
be appropriate to add the following to factor (1): . . . including the nature of the event being observed and the likelihood that the
witness would perceive, remember, and relate it correctly. State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (2006) (quoting State v. Ramirez,
817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)).
page98image30016 page98image30176 page98image30336 page98image30496 page98image30656 page98image30816 page98image30976 page98image31136 page98image31296 page98image31456 page98image31616 page98image31776 page98image31936 page98image32096 page98image32256
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include the witnesss ability to observe what he/she said he/she saw, the amount of time during which the witness saw the perpetrator, the distance from which the witness saw the perpetrator, and the lighting conditions at the time. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. 98, 114, 97 S.Ct. 2243, 2253 (1977); Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200-201, 93 S.Ct. at 382; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 239.
Where supported by evidence that the victim might have difficulty perceiving, recalling, or relating the events, it may
page98image37152 page98image37312 page98image37472 page98image37632 page98image37792 page98image37952 page98image38112 page98image38272 page98image38432 page98image38592 page98image38752 page98image38912
6
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether the witness was merely a passing or casual observer or one
who would be expected to pay scrupulous attention to detail, whether the witness was involved in a direct confrontation with the
perpetrator, whether the witness was nervous, shocked or scared as a result of any confrontation with the perpetrator, and whether
the witnesss attention was focused on or away from the perpetrators features. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97
S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93 S.Ct. at 382-383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240.
page98image45048 page98image45208 page98image45368
7
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after
page98image47048 page98image47208 page98image47368 page98image47528 page98image47688 page98image47848
observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided
details or was just general in nature, whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior
description of the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v.Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93
S.Ct. at 383; United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240-241; State v. Edmonds,
293 N.J. Super. 113 (App. Div. 1996).
8
page98image53256 page98image53416 page98image53576 page98image53736 page98image53896 page98image54056 page98image54216 page98image54376 page98image54536 page98image54696 page98image54856 page98image55016 page98image55176 page98image55336
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether witnesses making the identification received inadvertent or intentional confirmation, whether certainty was expressed at the time of the identification or some time later, whether intervening events following the identification affected the witnesss certainty, and whether the identification was made spontaneously and remained consistent thereafter. See N.J. Attorney General Guidelines for Preparing and Conducting Photo and Live Lineup
page98image58896 page98image59056
IDENTIFICATION:OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 3 of 5

(5) The length of time between the witnesss observation of the perpetrator during the offense and the identification.9
(6) The circumstances under which the identification was made, and whether or not it was the product of a suggestive procedure10, including everything done or said by law enforcement to the witness before, during, or after the identification process.11 In making this determination you may consider the following circumstances:
[REFER TO CIRCUMSTANCES OF THE IDENTIFICATION PROCEDURE AS NECESSARY FOR CLARITY, CHOOSING AS APPROPRIATE ANY OF THE FOLLOWING FACTORS, OR ANY OTHER FACTORS RELATING TO SUGGESTIVENESS, THAT ARE SUPPORTED BY THE EVIDENCE:]
  • whether anything was said to the witness prior to viewing a photo array, line-up or showup;12
  • whether a photo array shown to the witness contained multiple photographs of the defendant;13
  • whether all in the lineup but the [defendant] were known to the identifying witness;14
    Identification Procedures, April 18, 2001, at 2 (quoted in Herrera, 187 N.J. at 190); National Institute of Justice, Convicted by Juries, Exonerated by Science, June 1996, at 24 (available at https://www.ndjrs.gov/pdffiles/dnaevid.pdf); Gary Wells & Amy Bradfield, Good, You Identified the Suspect, 83 J. Applied Psychol. 360 (1998); Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781. Whether the witness made an identification quickly upon viewing the suspect, or whether the witness hesitated, may also be a relevant fact. See S. Sporer, Eyewitness Identification Accuracy, Confidence, and Decision Times in Simultaneous and Sequential Lineups, 78 J. Applied Psychol. 22, 23 (1993).
    Other relevant facts include whether, at a time prior to making the identification of this defendant, the witness either failed to identify the defendant or identified another person as the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at 383; Foster v. California, 394 U.S. 440, 442-443 & n.2, 89 S.Ct. 1127, 1128-1129 & n.2 (1969); United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 241. Madison cautions, with respect to an identification witnesss demonstrated certainty in his testimony, that a witnesss feeling of confidence in the details of memory generally do not validly measure the accuracy of the recollection, and that [i]n fact, witnesses frequently become more confident of the correctness of their memory over time while the actual memory trace is probably decaying. Id. at 241-242 (quoting W.LaFave and J.Israel, Criminal Procedure).
page99image21712 page99image21872 page99image22032 page99image22192 page99image22352 page99image22512 page99image22672 page99image22832 page99image22992 page99image23152 page99image23312 page99image23472 page99image23632 page99image23792 page99image23952 page99image24112 page99image24272 page99image24432 page99image24592 page99image24752 page99image24912 page99image25072 page99image25232 page99image25392 page99image25552 page99image25712 page99image25872 page99image26032 page99image26192 page99image26352 page99image26512
9
383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 242.
See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115-116, 97 S.Ct. at 2253-2254; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at
page99image28832 page99image28992 page99image29152 page99image29312 page99image29472 page99image29632 page99image29792 page99image29952 page99image30112
10
Refer to the New Jersey Attorney General Guidelines, footnote 8 supra. The court should focus on any allegations of
page99image31792 page99image31952
suggestive words or conduct by law enforcement or other persons that may effect the suggestiveness of the identification
procedures.
11
See State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493 (2006), in which the New Jersey Supreme Court addressed the propriety of a show-
page99image35096 page99image35256 page99image35416
up identification; the majority opinion concluded that, while such a procedure is inherently suggestive, the identification
procedure employed there was reliable and did not result in a substantial likelihood of misidentification.
12 13 14
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995). Id.
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 233, 87 S.Ct. at 1935.

page99image39656 page99image39816 page99image39976 page99image40136 page99image40296 page99image40456 page99image40616
IDENTIFICATION:OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 4 of 5

  • whether the other participants in a lineup were grossly dissimilar in appearance to the [defendant];15
  • whether only the [defendant] was required to wear distinctive clothing which the culprit allegedly wore;16
  • whether the witness is told by the police that they have caught the culprit after which the defendant is brought before the witness alone or is viewed in jail;17
  • whether the [defendant] is pointed out before or during a lineup;18
  • whether the witnesss identification was made spontaneously and remained consistent
    thereafter;19
  • whether the individual conducting the lineup either indicated to the witness that a suspect
    was present or failed to warn the witness that the perpetrator may or may not be in the
    procedure;20
  • whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions, or identifications given by
page100image9408 page100image9568 page100image9728
15 16 17 18 19 20
other witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other information or influence that may have affected the independence of his/her identification.21
[CHARGE IN ALL CASES:]
(7) Any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case which you consider relevant to your determination of whether the out-of-court identification was reliable.
[(8) Jury should be charged on any other relevant factor present in the case22]
Id.
Id.
Id.
Id., 87 S.Ct. at 1935-1936.
See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting State v. Ramirez, 817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)).
See N.J. Attorney Generals Guidelines, supra, Guideline I.B. (requiring administrator to instruct witness that

page100image17384 page100image17544 page100image17704 page100image17864 page100image18024 page100image18184 page100image18344 page100image18504 page100image18664 page100image18824 page100image18984 page100image19144 page100image19304
perpetrator may not be present); State v. Ledbetter, 881 A.2d 290 (Ct. 2005) (requiring jury instruction to that effect).
21
22
page100image21144 page100image21304
See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781 n. 2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n. 8 (Utah 1986)).
page100image23016 page100image23176 page100image23336 page100image23496 page100image23656 page100image23816 page100image23976 page100image24136
The list of factors enumerated in Biggers and Madison is not exhaustive. See State v. White, 158 N.J. 230, (1999) (in declining to find plain error in identification charge, court notes that instruction went beyond model charge, noting the discrepancy ... between identifications made by different witnesses). Additional relevant factors that should be brought to jurys attention include the witnesss inability to make an in-court identification if asked to do so while on the witness stand, any failure on the part of the State to record a line-up or preserve a photo array, as bearing upon the probative value of the out-of-court identification, see State v. Delgado, 188 N.J. 48, 63 (2006); State v. Earle, 60 N.J. 550, 552 (1972); State v. Peterkin, 226 N.J. Super. 25, 46 (App. Div. 1988), and any discrepancies between identifications made by different witnesses, State v. White, 158
page100image31088 page100image31248 page100image31408 page100image31568 page100image31728 page100image31888 page100image32048 page100image32208 page100image32368 page100image32528 page100image32688 page100image32848 page100image33008 page100image33168
IDENTIFICATION:OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 5 of 5

[IN THE APPROPRIATE CASE,23 CHARGE THE FOLLOWING FACTOR:]
(9) The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.24
[CHARGE IN ALL CASES:]
Unless the out-of-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the out-of-court identification procedure, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of the identification is for you to decide.
If, after considering all the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after considering all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
page101image12200 page101image12360 page101image12520
N.J. 230, 248.
23
An instruction that cross-racial identification is a factor to be considered should be given only when ... identification is
page101image14728
a critical issue in the case, and an eyewitnesss cross-racial identification is not corroborated by other evidence giving it
independent reliability. State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. at 132; see also State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007).
24
page101image17248 page101image17408 page101image17568 page101image17728 page101image17888 page101image18048
Cromedy holds that in order for the jury to determine the reliability of a cross-racial identification not corroborated by independent evidence, the jury must be informed of the potential risks associated with such identifications, that the jury must be instructed about the possible significance of the cross-racial identification factor.... 158 N.J. at 132-33. In State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007), the New Jersey Supreme Court refused to rule that cross-ethnic charges were required in cases involving an individuals identification of a person of another ethnic background.
page101image22512 page101image22672 page101image22832 page101image22992
Consequences of a Criminal Guilty Plea
1. You will have to appear in open court and tell the judge what you did that makes you guilty of the particular offense(s)
2. Do you understand that if you plead guilty:
a. You will have a criminal record
b. You may go to Jail or Prison.
c. You will have to pay Fines and Court Costs.
3. If you are on Probation, you will have to submit to random drug and urine testing. If you violate Probation, you often go to jail.
4. In indictable matters, you will be required to provide a DNA sample, which could be used by law enforcement for the investigation of criminal activity, and pay for the cost of testing.
5. You must pay restitution if the court finds there is a victim who has suffered a loss and if the court finds that you are able or will be able in the future to pay restitution.
6. If you are a public office holder or employee, you can be required to forfeit your office or job by virtue of your plea of guilty.
7. If you are not a United States citizen or national, you may be deported by virtue of your plea of guilty.
8. You must wait 5-10 years to expunge a first offense. 2C:52-3
9. You could be put on Probation.
10. In Drug Cases, a mandatory DEDR penalty of $500-$1,000, and lose your drivers license for 6 months - 2 years. You must pay a Law Enforcement Officers Training and Equipment Fund penalty of $30.
11. You may be required to do Community Service.
12. You must pay a minimum Violent Crimes Compensation Board assessment of $50 ($100 minimum if you are convicted of a crime of violence) for each count to which you plead guilty.
13. You must pay a $75 Safe Neighborhood Services Fund assessment for each conviction.
14. If you are being sentenced to probation, you must pay a fee of up to $25 per month for the term of probation.
15. You lose the presumption against incarceration in future cases. 2C:44-1
16. You may lose your right to vote.
The defense of a person charged with a criminal offense is not impossible. There are a number of viable defenses and arguments which can be pursued to achieve a successful result. Advocacy, commitment, and persistence are essential to defending a client accused of a criminal offense.
Jail for Crimes and Disorderly Conduct:
If someone pleads Guilty or is found Guilty of a criminal offense, the following is the statutory Prison/Jail terms.
NJSA 2C: 43-8 (1) In the case of a crime of the first degree, for a specific term of years which shall be fixed by the court and shall be between 10 years and 20 years;
(2) In the case of a crime of the second degree, for a specific term of years which shall be fixed by the court and shall be between five years and 10 years;
(3) In the case of a crime of the third degree, for a specific term of years which shall be fixed by the court and shall be between three years and five years;
(4) In the case of a crime of the fourth degree, for a specific term which shall be fixed by the court and shall not exceed 18 months.
2C:43-3 Fines have been increased recently! 2C:43-3. Fines and Restitutions. A person who has been convicted of an offense may be sentenced to pay a fine, to make restitution, or both, such fine not to exceed:
a. (1) $200,000.00 when the conviction is of a crime of the first degree;
(2) $150,000.00 when the conviction is of a crime of the second degree;
b. (1) $15,000.00 when the conviction is of a crime of the third degree;
(2) $10,000.00 when the conviction is of a crime of the fourth degree;
c. $1,000.00, when the conviction is of a disorderly persons offense;
d. $500.00, when the conviction is of a petty disorderly persons offense;
If facing any criminal charge, retain an experienced attorney immediately to determine you rights and obligations to the court. Current criminal charge researched by Kenneth Vercammen, Esq. 732-572-0500

No comments: