IDENTIFICATION report & lineups in NJ
REPORT OF THE SUPREME COURT COMMITTEE ON MODEL CRIMINAL JURY CHARGES ON THE REVISIONS TO THE IDENTIFICATION MODEL CHARGES
Approved by Supreme Court Committee on Model Criminal Jury Charges January 9, 2012
TABLE OF CONTENTS
PAGE I. OVERVIEW ......................................................................................................... 1 II REVISION OF IDENTIFICATION MODEL JURY CHARGES ............................2 III. IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS CHARGE.....................3
i
PAGE
1. 2. 3.
M. Prior 1.
Courts
Findings
.................................................................... 25
Committees Discussion
.............................................................. 25
Recommended
Paragraph...........................................................25
Description
of Perpetrator Factor
................................................... 26 Committees
Discussion
..............................................................26
Recommended
Paragraph...........................................................27
2.
...........................................................
28 .......................................................... 28
.......................................................... 28
.......................................................... 29
.......................................................... 30
.......................................................... 30
.......................................................... 30
.......................................................... 31
.......................................................... 32
.......................................................... 32
.......................................................... 32
.......................................................... 33
.......................................................... 34
Q. Catch-all Factor
R.
Inconsistent Statements Factor
.......................................................... 36 1.
Committees Discussion
..........................................................36
..........................................................
40 .......................................................... 40
.......................................................... 41
.......................................................... 41
.......................................................... 42
.......................................................... 42
.......................................................... 43
.......................................................... 46
ii
PAGE
Proposed Charges
Current Charges
iii
I. OVERVIEW
In State v. Henderson, 208 N.J. 208, 298 (2011), the Court asked the Criminal Practice Committee and the Model Criminal Jury Charge Committee to draft proposed revisions to the current charges on eyewitness identification and submit them to the Court for approval. Based upon the scientific evidence in the record the Court was convinced: [M]emory is malleable, and that an array of variables can affect and dilute memory and lead to misidentifications. Those factors include system variables like lineup procedures, which are within the control of the criminal justice system, and estimator variables like lighting conditions or the presence of a weapon, over which the legal system has no control. [ Id. at 218.] Therefore, the Court concluded: [T]he current standard for assessing eyewitness identification evidence does not fully meet its goals. It does not offer an adequate measure for reliability or sufficiently deter inappropriate police conduct. It also overstates the jurys inherent ability to evaluate evidence offered by eyewitnesses who honestly believe their testimony is accurate. [ Ibid.] In order to remedy its concerns regarding the jury charges, the Court
requested that:
[T]he
court system ... develop enhanced jury charges on eyewitness
identification for trial judges to use. We anticipate that
identification evidence will continue to be admitted in the vast
majority of cases. To help jurors weigh that evidence, they must be told
about relevant factors and their effect on reliability...With the use
of more focused jury charges on those issues, there will be less need to
call expert witnesses at trial. Trial courts will still have discretion
to admit expert testimony when warranted.
[ Id. at 218-19.]
1
II. REVISION OF IDENTIFICATION MODEL JURY CHARGES
Scientific literature divides the variables that can affect and dilute memory and lead to misidentification into two categories: System and Estimator variables. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 247. In calling for a revision of the Identification model jury charges, the Court charged the Committees to consider all of the system and estimator variables in section VI for which we have found scientific support that is generally accepted by experts.... Id. at 298-99. In response to this charge, the Model Criminal Jury Charge Committee formed a subcommittee to draft revisions of the Identification charges for the full Committees review. The Committee reviewed and modified the subcommittees proposed revision, using the In-Court and Out-of-Court Identifications charge as a template. The charge was unanimously approved by the Committee. The Committee decided that the language approved in that charge could then be incorporated into the other two Identification charges: In-Court Identification Only and Out-ofCourt Identification Only. The Committees discussion regarding each section of the proposed In-Court and Out-of-Court Identifications charge, and the Courts findings on the factors have been incorporated into the following summary. Comparisons to the current charge have also been included where applicable. See Appendix A for the proposed
charges and Appendix B
for the current charges.
2
III. IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS CHARGE
A. First Two Paragraphs 1. Committees Discussion Because the first two paragraphs conform to the current In-Court and Out-of Court Identifications charge, the only objection raised by the Committee concerned the inclusion of the phrase beyond a reasonable doubt in the last sentence of the second paragraph on page 2. That sentence in the original version stated: You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. [Emphasis added.] The Committee was divided on this issue. Some members asserted that this language is not included in the current charge, and therefore it should not be included in this charge either. They were of the view that since the first paragraph includes beyond a reasonable doubt it may be confusing to jurors to again include the burden in this paragraph. Additionally, those members pointed out that this burden was stated again in the first sentence of the paragraph on page 3. That sentence stated: To decide whether the identification testimony is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged, you should evaluate the testimony of the witness in light of the factors for considering credibility that I have already explained to you. [Emphasis added.]
3
Ultimately,
the Committee agreed to delete the phrase upon which to conclude beyond
a reasonable doubt in both of these sentences.
2. Recommended Language The following was approved for the first two paragraphs: (Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find this defendant guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed the crime. The defendant has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proven beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed it. The State has presented the testimony of [insert name of witness who identified defendant]. You will recall that this witness identified the defendant in court as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. The State also presented testimony that on a prior occasion before this trial, this witness identified the defendant as the person who committed this offense [these offenses]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the witnesss identification of the defendant is reliable and believable, or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged.
1
86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981); State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v. Green,
4
B. Inclusion of References to Scientific Studies and Research
1. Committees Discussion The subcommittee could not reach a consensus on whether specific references to the social science studies and scientific research included in Henderson should be noted in the third paragraph and in some of the factors, such as stress. For example the third paragraph originally proposed by the subcommittee members in support of including these references stated: Scientific research has amply demonstrated the dangers of mistake in human perception and identification. That research has focused on the nature of memory and the factors that affect the reliability of eyewitness identification evidence. Those members stressed the importance of including the detailed description of the scientific findings and research found by the Court in Henderson. They were of the view that the jury should not only be told about the variables but also understand the science on how such variables can effect a witnesss memory. On the other hand, the opposing members of the subcommittee stressed that the Committee needs to be aware that the social science research is probabilistic in that it cannot determine that a witness is right or wrong in his or her identification. However, those members acknowledged that certain factors may exist that should be explained to the jurors to help them determine the accuracy of the identification, and whether the State proved the identification beyond a reasonable doubt. Further, those members recognized that the jury
5
needs to know that the presence of certain factors may increase the chance of misidentification.
The Committee had a lengthy discussion on this issue. It was recognized that the cross-racial identification charge required in State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112 (1999), does not reference the social science studies although the charge is based upon those findings. Most members agreed that the charge should not reference the social science studies or use the term scientific research. It was the general consensus that the charge should focus on the Courts findings as it relates to the jurys consideration of the reliability of the identification, and that the presence of certain factors may increase the chance of misidentification. For example, if the lighting is bad the charge does not need to state that the science shows... Those members who initially opposed deleting these references ultimately withdrew their objections. Therefore, the Committee agreed to delete all references to the social science studies and scientific research in the charge.
6
C. Inclusion of Detailed Description on Memory and its Stages 1. Committees Discussion
The third and fifth paragraphs originally proposed by some members of the subcommittee included a detailed description on memory and its stages as set forth in Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 245-48. However, the subcommittee could not reach a consensus on including this language. In particular, the original language in the third paragraph proposed by the supporting subcommittee members stated: Eyewitness identification evidence must be scrutinized carefully. Scientific research has amply demonstrated the dangers of mistake in human perception and identification. That research has focused on the nature of memory and the factors that affect the reliability of eyewitness identification evidence. I will instruct you on the specific factors you should consider in this case in determining whether the eyewitness identification evidence is reliable. Their fifth paragraph originally stated: In evaluating the reliability of the identification, you should understand what science has demonstrated about how human memory works. Human memory is not at all like a video recording that a witness need only replay to remember what happened. Human memory is far more complex. Memory or the process of remembering consists of three stages: acquisition (the perception of the original event); retention (the period of time that passes between the event and the eventual recollection of a piece of information); and retrieval (the stage during which a person recalls stored information). At each of these stages, information that ultimately forms the memory can be distorted, contaminated, and even falsely imagined. Unlike a videotape, the witness does not perceive everything that occurs; rather, the witness constructs a memory based on pieces of information perceived at the time of the incident. In other words, the witness does not encode or record all the information in the same way that a videotape does. Additionally, whereas a
7
videotape remains intact, memory is malleable and can unknowingly be contaminated by a variety of factors.
The subcommittee members who proposed that language asserted that since the Court adopted the scientific research on memory, it should be included in the charge. Further those members were of the view that the first two sentences in the third paragraph go to the heart of the Henderson case. Additionally, those members asserted that the language in the fifth paragraph on how memory works was, in their view, the core of the scientific research in this area. They emphasized the importance of the jury understanding the sentence that memory is not like a video recording. The opposing members of the subcommittee asserted that the fifth paragraph purported to instruct the jury on how memory works, which in their view was a subject beyond the scope of a jury instruction. They considered that paragraph essentially a lecture comparing human memory to videotape and filled with loaded words such as distorted, contaminated, false, and malleable. They were strongly opposed to including this language. Those members suggested that the charge instead state: Human beings do have the ability to recognize other people from past experiences and to identify them at a later time. But human memory is not foolproof. One Committee member did not like the term foolproof. Other options were raised for this term but ultimately the Committee decided to keep foolproof. The Committee held a lengthy discussion on whether to include the detailed description on memory and its stages. It was expressed that there is a
8
difference
in including language in a court opinion and a charge. Overall, the
general consensus was that the charge did not need to include a
dissertation on memory and its stages. Further, the Committee was of the
view that the charge should refrain from using too much description on
memory.
Those members who initially supported including this language withdrew their objections. Therefore, the Committee agreed for the effectiveness of the charge that there should be some preliminary information on memory. The Committee decided to include that memory is not like a video recording and that human memory is far more complex. However, the Committee agreed to delete the remaining portion of the fifth paragraph concerning the detailed description on memory and its stages. 2. Recommended Third Paragraph The Committee approved the following: Eyewitness identification evidence must be scrutinized carefully. Human beings do have the ability to recognize other people from past experiences and to identify them at a later time. But human memory is not foolproof. Research has shown that human memory is not at all like a video recording that a witness need only replay to remember what happened. Human memory is far more complex.2 I will instruct you on specific factors you should consider in this case in determining whether the eyewitness identification evidence is reliable. State v. Henderson, 208 N.J. 208, 245 (2011).
2
9
D. Fourth and Fifth Paragraphs on Evaluating the Identification
1. Committees Discussion There were no objections to the language in these two paragraphs, which except for some minor changes is from the second and third paragraphs of the current charge.
3 4
2. Recommended Language
The Committee approved the following for these paragraphs: In evaluating this identification, you should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, the witnesss ability to make those observations and perceptions, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.3 In evaluating the identifications, you should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification were based, and the witnesss ability to make those observations and perceptions. If you determine that the out-of-court identification is not reliable, you may still consider the witnesss in-court identification of the defendant if you find it to be reliable. Unless the in-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the out-of-court identification procedure, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issues of the reliability of both the in-court and out-of-court identifications is for you to decide.4 State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007).
United
States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 229-232, 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1933-1935, 1940
(manner in which lineup or other identification procedure conducted
relevant to reliability of out-of-court identification and in-court
identification following out-of-court identification, and jurys
credibility determinations).
10
E. Introductory Paragraph for the Estimator Factors
The charge first addresses the Estimator variables. These are the factors beyond the control of the criminal justice system. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 261. Moreover, the Court explained: They can include factors related to the incident, the witness, or the perpetrator. Estimator variables are equally capable of affecting an eyewitness ability to perceive and remember an event. Although the factors can be isolated and tested in lab experiments, they occur at random in the real world. [ Ibid.] It should be noted that like the current charge the term factors is used rather than variables. In addition, except where noted below the paragraphs describing the factors are new to the charge. 1. Committees Discussion This paragraph is the same as the current charge, except for the addition of the third sentence, which states, In addition you should consider the following factors that are related to the witness, the alleged perpetrator of the crime, and the criminal incident itself. One area of disagreement among the subcommittee members was the use of may versus should in regards to the jurys consideration of the factors in this paragraph. The subcommittee members supporting the use of should argued that in their opinion the research based variables that affect reliability as discussed in Henderson require the fact finder to consider the factors that science has demonstrated affect reliability because they are based on research.
11
The
subcommittee members who preferred the use of may explained that the
use of should risks directing jury deliberations, while the use of may
draws attention to the findings but permits the jury to decide whether
they should be applied. Those subcommittee members emphasized that there
is a danger of channeling the jurors discretion by the use of should
rather than may.
The Committee was divided on this issue. It was expressed that the current charge uses should, and the Court did not recommend changing that language. By a close vote, the Committee agreed to use should. 2. Recommended Paragraph The Committee approved the following: To decide whether the identification testimony is sufficiently reliable evidence that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged, you should evaluate the testimony of the witness in light of the factors for considering credibility that I have already explained to you. In addition, you should consider the following factors that are related to the witness, the alleged perpetrator of the crime, and the criminal incident itself.5 In particular, you should consider [choose appropriate factors from one through six below]:
5
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 247.
12
F. The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention Factor
1. Committees Discussion This language, except for some minor changes, is from the first two factors of the current charge on page 3. 2. Recommended Paragraph The Committee approved the following for this factor: (1) The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention: In evaluating the reliability of the identification, you should assess the witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense and the witnesss degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense. In making this assessment you should consider the following [choose appropriate factors from (a) through (i) below]:
13
G. Stress Factor
1. Courts Findings In Henderson, the Court found that high levels of stress are likely to affect the reliability of eyewitness identifications. Id. at 262. However, the Court noted that There is no precise measure for what constitutes high stress, which must be assessed based on the facts presented in individual cases. Ibid. 2. Committees Discussion The original version reviewed by the Committee stated: Stress: Scientific research has proven that even under the best viewing conditions, highly stressful events can have a negative effect on memory and increase the risk of a mistaken identification. Therefore, you should consider the level of the witness stress and whether the stressful nature of the event distracted the witness or made it harder for him or her to identify the perpetrator. The Committee first deleted the phrase Scientific research has proven in the first sentence. The Committee then recognized the importance of the statement in Henderson that [e]ven under the best viewing conditions, high levels of stress can diminish an eyewitness ability to recall and make an accurate identification. Id. at 261. Therefore, the Committee agreed to revise the first sentence to conform to that statement. The Committee next agreed to delete the phrase in the last sentence concerning the stressful nature of the event.
14
3. Recommended Paragraph
The paragraph approved by the Committee is as follows: (a) Stress: Even under the best viewing conditions, high levels of stress can reduce an eyewitnesss ability to recall and make an accurate identification. Therefore, you should consider the level of the witnesss stress and whether that stress, if any, distracted the witness or made it harder for him or her to identify the perpetrator. 6
6
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 261-62.
15
H. Weapons Focus Factor 1. Courts Findings
For this factor, the Court explained that When a visible weapon is used during a crime, it can distract a witness and draw his or her attention away from the culprit. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 262. Further, the Court stated:
The original version reviewed by the Committee stated: Weapons Focus: You should consider whether a weapon was visible to the witness during the incident. The presence of a weapon can distract the witness and take the witness attention away from the perpetrators face. As a result, the presence of a visible weapon may reduce the reliability of a subsequent identification. In considering this factor, you should take into account the duration of the crime because the longer the duration of the event, the more time the witness may have to adapt to the presence of the weapon and focus on other details. The Committee was mindful of the Courts findings in Henderson that when the interaction is brief, the presence of a visible weapon can affect the reliability of an identification and the accuracy of a witness description of the perpetrator. Id. at 263.
16
Further,
the Committee agreed that this factor will not apply in every case
where there is a visible weapon, i.e. it will only apply if the crime is
of short duration. Therefore, the Committee decided to add that phrase
at the end of the third sentence.
3. Recommended Paragraph
The Committee approved the following paragraph: (b) Weapons Focus: You should consider whether the witness saw the weapon during the incident. The presence of a weapon can distract the witness and take the witnesss attention away from the perpetrators face. As a result, the presence of a visible weapon may reduce the reliability of a subsequent identification if the crime is of a short duration. In considering this factor, you should take into account the duration of the crime because the longer the duration of the event, the more time the witness may have to adapt to the presence of the weapon and focus on other details.7
7
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 262-63.
17
I. Duration Factor
1. Courts Findings The Court found that Not surprisingly, the amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. Id. at 264. Further, the Court noted that Whatever the threshold, studies have shown, and the Special Master found, that witnesses consistently tend to overestimate short durations, particularly where much was going on or the event was particularly stressful. Ibid. 2. Committees Discussion The original language reviewed by the Committee for this factor stated: Duration: The amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. A brief or fleeting observation is less likely to produce an accurate identification than a more prolonged exposure. In addition, scientific research has shown that the time estimate given by a witness may not always be accurate because witnesses tend to think events lasted longer than they actually did. The Committee was mindful of the importance of including the statement in Henderson that There is no measure to determine exactly how long a view is needed to be able to make a reliable identification. Ibid. Therefore, the Committee agreed to add Although there is no minimum time required to make an accurate identification at the beginning of the second sentence. In addition, the phrase to the perpetrator was added at the end of that sentence.
18
3. Recommended Paragraph
The Committee approved the following paragraph: (c) Duration: The amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. Although there is no minimum time required to make an accurate identification a brief or fleeting contact is less likely to produce an accurate identification than a more prolonged exposure to the perpetrator. In addition, the time estimate given by a witness may not always be accurate because witnesses tend to think events lasted longer than they actually did. 8
8
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 264.
19
J. Distance and Lighting Factors 1. Courts Findings
The Henderson Court found: It is obvious that a person is easier to recognize when close by, and that clarity decreases with distance. We also know that poor lighting makes it harder to see well. Thus, greater distance between a witness and a perpetrator and poor lighting conditions can diminish the reliability of an identification. [ Id. at 264.] The Court also noted Research has shown that people have difficulty estimating distances. Ibid. 2. Committees Discussion The third sentence in the original version for the Distance factor stated In addition, scientific research has shown that a witness estimate of how far he was from the perpetrator may not always be accurate because people tend to have difficulty estimating distances. The only change made by the Committee to this paragraph was to delete the phrase scientific research has shown in that sentence. With regards to the Lighting factor, the original language stated: Lighting: Poor lighting makes it harder to see well, and therefore, poor lighting conditions can diminish the reliability of an identification. The Committee considered the Courts findings that poor lighting conditions can diminish the reliability of an identification. Various alternatives were discussed for the term poor, but the Committee ultimately agreed on inadequate.
20
The
Committee also decided to add You should consider the lighting
conditions that were present at the time of the alleged crime in this
case.
3. Recommended Paragraphs
The approved language states: (d) Distance: A person is easier to recognize when close by. The greater the distance between an eyewitness and a perpetrator, the higher the risk of a mistaken identification. In addition, a witnesss estimate of how far he or she was from the perpetrator may not always be accurate because people tend to have difficulty estimating distances. 9 (e) Lighting: Inadequate lighting can reduce the reliability of an identification. You should consider the lighting conditions that were present at the time of the alleged crime in this case.10
9 10
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 264. Ibid.
21
K. Intoxication Factor
1. Courts Findings The Court found that the level of intoxication can affect the reliability of an identification. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 265. In support of this conclusion the Court noted the findings of the Special Master that the effects of alcohol on identification accuracy show that high levels of alcohol promote false identifications and that low alcohol intake produces fewer misidentifications than high alcohol intake. Ibid. 2. Committees Discussion In crafting the language for this factor, the subcommittee could not reach a consensus. The version proposed by some members of the subcommittee stated: Intoxication: An identification given by a witness who was highly impaired or intoxicated at the time of the incident tends to be more unreliable than an identification given by a sober witness. The opposing members of the subcommittee pointed out that Henderson only referenced alcohol and not narcotics or prescription drugs. Those members noted that the description of this factor in Henderson does not include any findings or discussion on whether prescription drugs or narcotics have an effect on the identification and whether the jury should be advised on them. Moreover, they asserted that Henderson is not just tied to intoxication; rather the opinion ties the reliability of the identification to the level of intoxication.
22
The
Committee first considered whether jurors would understand the term
intoxication and whether to instead reference under the influence. The
Committee could not reach a consensus on which term was better.
The Committee then noted the importance of the findings of the Special Master that high levels of alcohol promote false identifications, whereas low alcohol intake produces fewer misidentifications than high alcohol intake. Therefore, the Committee agreed to include that phrase rather than highly impaired or intoxicated. Some Committee members asserted that the charge should include references to any evidence, including drugs that might have an effect on a witness such as alcohol or drugs. After a lengthy discussion, a majority of the Committee approved adding a sentence to the footnote, which would state that Although Henderson only addressed alcohol if there is evidence of impairment by drugs or other substances the parties may want to have the jury consider this issue. After the Committee meeting, some members indicated that they were going to raise an issue at the next meeting that the Intoxication factor should be revisited because one of the Estimator variables that should be evaluated during a pre-trial hearing after suggestiveness has been shown is, [w]as the witness under the influence of alcohol or drugs. Therefore, they planned to suggest that this factor be revised to state a witness who is highly intoxicated and/or whether the witness was under the influence of alcohol or drugs. However, those members did not raise this issue at the next meeting, nor were there any
23
additional objections raised concerning this language in the final approved charge.
3. Recommended Paragraph
The language approved by the Committee states: (f) Intoxication: An identification given by a witness with a high level of alcohol at the time of the incident tends to be more unreliable than an identification given by a witness who did not have a high level of alcohol.11
11
Henderson,
supra, 208 N.J. at 265. Although Henderson only addressed alcohol if
there is evidence of impairment by drugs or other substances the parties
may want to have the jury consider this issue.
24
L. Disguises/Changed Appearance Factor 1. Courts Findings
The Court stated, Disguises and changes in facial features can affect a witness ability to remember and identify a perpetrator. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 266. Further the Court recognized the Special Masters findings that [d]isguises (e.g., hats, sunglasses, masks) are confounding to witnesses and reduce the accuracy of identifications. Ibid. Additionally, the Court found that disguises as simple as hats have been shown to reduce identification accuracy. Ibid. Moreover, the Court noted If facial features are altered between the time of the event and the identification procedure if for example, the culprit grows a beardthe accuracy of an identification may decrease. Ibid.
12 13
2. Committees Discussion
The Committee approved the language as proposed by the subcommittee. 3. Recommended Paragraph The approved language states: (g) Disguises/Changed Appearance: The perpetrators use of a disguise can affect a witnesss ability to both remember and identify the perpetrator. Disguises can be as simple as hats or sunglasses, as well as masks, and can reduce the accuracy of an identification.12 Similarly, if facial features are altered between the time of the event and a subsequent identification procedure, the accuracy of the identification may decrease.13 Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 266. Ibid.
25
M. Prior Description of Perpetrator Factor 1. Committees Discussion
The subcommittee could not reach a consensus on the language for this factor. The paragraph proposed by some members of the subcommittee stated: Prior Description of Perpetrator: Another important factor for your consideration is the accuracy of any description of the perpetrator that the witness gave prior to the identification procedure. Consider whether any description given prior to the identification procedure accurately matches the photo or person picked out during the procedure. Additionally, you should give careful consideration to whether the witness testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his or her prior description of the perpetrator. Finally, you should consider whether the prior description provided details or was just general in nature because a witness inability to provide a detailed description of the perpetrator may call into question the reliability of the witness identification The opposing members of the subcommittee asserted that the sentence whether any description given prior to the identification procedure accurately matches the photo or person picked out during the procedure was not in Henderson. The Committee discussed this issue and ultimately agreed to use the language in the current charge on page 3, which advises the jury that they may consider the accuracy of any description the witness gave prior to identifying the perpetrator. In addition, the Committee decided that the first sentence of the footnote in the current charge should be included in this paragraph, and to delete the rest of the footnote.
26
2. Recommended Paragraph
The language approved by the Committee states: (2) Prior Description of Perpetrator: Another factor for your consideration is the accuracy of any description the witness gave before identifying the perpetrator. Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided details or was just general in nature, and whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator.
27
N. Confidence and Accuracy Factor 1. Courts Findings
With regards to highly confident witnesses, the Court recognized its conclusion in State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007), that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 254. Further, the Henderson Court noted the Special Masters findings that eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy, but he acknowledged research showing that highly confident witnesses can make accurate identifications 90% of the time. Ibid. 2. Committees Discussion The subcommittee could not reach a consensus on this paragraph. The version proposed by some members of the subcommittee stated: Confidence and Accuracy: You heard testimony that (insert name of witness) made a statement at the time of his/her identification of defendant from the photo array/line-up concerning his/her level of certainty that the photograph he/she selected is in fact the person who committed the crime. A witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification. Although some scientific research has found that highly confident witnesses are more likely to make accurate identifications, eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy. One Committee member expressed concerns with the sentence that Although some scientific research has found that highly confident witnesses are more likely to make accurate identifications, eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy. That member was concerned that this
28
statement bolsters the testimony of the States witness. The Committee initially considered deleting that sentence.
However, the subcommittee member who proposed that sentence explained that it was drafted to balance the language in Henderson that a confidence statement taken at the time of the out-of-court identification without confirmatory feedback and recorded would be relevant at the trial. In his view, if the statement was not recorded it could not be used at trial. However, it was expressed that a confidence statement is only relevant if it was made at the time of the identification. After a lengthy discussion, the Committee decided to delete the phrase scientific in that sentence. In addition, the Committee agreed to delete the sentence that A witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.
14
3. Recommended Paragraph
The language approved by the Committee states: (3) Confidence and Accuracy: You heard testimony that (insert name of witness) made a statement at the time of his/her identification of defendant from the photo array/line-up concerning his/her level of certainty that the photograph he/she selected is in fact the person who committed the crime. Although some research has found that highly confident witnesses are more likely to make accurate identifications, eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy.14 Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 253-55.
29
O. Time Elapsed Factor
1. Courts Findings With regards to memory decay, the Court stated: Memories fade with time. And as the Special Master observed, memory decay is irreversible; memories never improve. As a result, delays between the commission of a crime and the time an identification is made can affect reliability. That basic principle is not in dispute. [ See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 267.] Further, the Court stated, In other words, the more time that passes, the greater the possibility that a witness memory of a perpetrator will weaken. Ibid. 2. Committees Discussion The first three sentences proposed by some members of the subcommittee stated: Scientific evidence has proven that memories fade with time. This process is not reversible. Once a memory fades or decays it can never be recovered. However, the subcommittee could not reach a consensus on including these sentences. The subcommittee members opposed to this language asserted that memory can be recalled. In reviewing this paragraph, the Committee first deleted the reference to scientific evidence has proven that in the first sentence. The Committee then considered the statement in Henderson that researchers cannot pinpoint precisely when a persons recall becomes unreliable. Id. at 267. After a lengthy discussion, the Committee agreed to retain the Memories fade with time sentence and delete the second and third sentences.
30
3. Recommended Paragraph
The language approved by the Committee states: (4) Time Elapsed: Memories fade with time. As a result, delays between the commission of a crime and the time an identification is made can affect the reliability of the identification. In other words, the more time that passes, the greater the possibility that a witnesss memory of a perpetrator will weaken.15
15
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 267.
31
P. Cross-Racial Effects Factor 1. Courts Findings
With regards to cross-racial identification the Henderson Court citing to State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 120 (1999) explained: A cross-racial identification occurs when an eyewitness is asked to identify a person of another race. Cromedy, supra, 158 N.J. at 120. In Cromedy, after citing multiple social science sources, this Court recognized that a witness may have more difficulty making a cross-racial identification. [citation omitted]. [Id. at 267.] Further, the Court stated that [c]ross-racial recognition continues to be a factor that can affect the reliability of an identification. Ibid. As to the charge for cross-racial identification, the Court advised: In 1999, the Court in Cromedy directed that the charge be given only when . . . identification is a critical issue in the case, and an eyewitnesss cross-racial identification is not corroborated by other evidence giving it independent reliability. Cromedy, supra, 158 N.J. at 132. Since then, the additional research on own-race bias discussed in section VI.B.8, and the more complete record about eye-witness identification in general, justify giving the charge whenever cross-racial identification is in issue at trial. [ Id. at 299.] 2. Committees Discussion The original paragraph reviewed by the Committee stated: Cross-Racial Effects: The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant is a significant factor that may influence the accuracy of the witness identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race. In fact,
32
cross-racial identifications have been proven to be more unreliable than same-race identifications.
The Committee first turned to the language in the current charge on page 6, and compared it to the proposed paragraph. The Committee agreed to use the language in the current charge, and to add in the footnote that this instruction must rather than should be given whenever there is a cross-racial identification.
3. Recommended Paragraph
The language approved by the Committee states: (5) Cross-Racial Effects: The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race. 16
16
This
instruction must be given whenever there is a cross-racial
identification. Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 299 (modifying State v.
Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 132 (1999).
33
Q. Catch-all Factor
1. OriginalFactorsonChildWitnessesandOwn-Age Bias Some members of the subcommittee proposed including separate factors on Child Witnesses and Own-Age Bias, which stated the following: Child Witnesses: Identifications made by young children must be scrutinized carefully. Child eyewitnesses under the age of thirteen are more likely to make incorrect identifications than adults. Own-Age Bias: Scientific evidence has shown that witnesses are better at recognizing people of their own age than people of other ages. However, the subcommittee could not reach a consensus on whether the Court made specific findings on these factors, and whether they should be included in the charge. In particular, the subcommittee members opposed to including these factors asserted that Henderson did not find that identifications made by young children must be scrutinized carefully. Further, those members asserted that the Special Masters finding was that [a] witnesss age ... bears on the reliability of an identification. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 265. Moreover, they pointed out that the research showed that children under thirteen were more likely to make incorrect identifications than adults when they view target-absent line-ups. Ibid. As to the Own-Age Bias factor, the Committee considered the testimony of Dr. Penrod that witnesses are better at recognizing people of [their] own age than... people of other ages and that this may appear in studies that use college-age students as targets. Id. at 265-66. Additionally, it was asserted that
34
Henderson
also referenced that Perhaps people should only use age as a factor in
deciding whether to believe an eyewitness if there is a large age
difference between the witness and the suspect. Id. at 266.
The Committee initially agreed to delete the first sentence in the Child Witnesses factor that Identifications made by young children must be scrutinized carefully. However, it was then expressed that the second sentence was not an accurate statement. After further discussion, the Committee considered whether to have a Catch-all factor at the end of the section on Estimator variables, similar to factor 9 in the current charge, to incorporate the Courts findings that characteristics like a witness age...can affect the reliability of an identification. Id. at 265. Factor 9 in the current charge states that the Jury should be charged on any other relevant factor present in the case. Most members agreed to include the Catch-all factor, and delete the Child Witnesses and Own-Age Bias factors. Those members that were opposed to deleting these factors did not renew their objections when the charges were ultimately approved. 2. RecommendedParagraph The language approved by the Committee states: (6) Catch-all Factor: You may also consider the age of the identifying witness and any other relevant characteristics of the witness.
35
R. Inconsistent Statements Factor 1. Committees Discussion
The original draft included an Inconsistent Statements factor, stated: Inconsistent Statements: In evaluating the identification, you should consider any discrepancies or inconsistencies between the witness trial testimony and the out-of-court identification(s). (The footnote at the end of this paragraph cited to State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118 (App. Div. 2000)).
which
Initially, the Committee approved the language in this factor, which is from factor 6 of the current charge.
However, upon reviewing the final version of the charge, the Committee decided that this factor was too repetitive with the Prior Description of Perpetrator factor. Therefore, the Committee agreed to delete the Inconsistent Statements factor.
36
S. Introductory Paragraph for the Systems Factors
These are factors such as lineup procedures, which are within the control of the criminal justice system. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 247. 1. Committees Discussion There were no changes made by the Committee to the language proposed by the subcommittee.
2. Recommended Paragraph
The approved language states: In evaluating the reliability of a witnesss identification, you should also consider the circumstances under which any out-of-court identification was made, and whether or not it was the result of a suggestive procedure, including everything done or said by law enforcement to the witness during the identification process. You should consider the following factors: [Charge if appropriate]:17
17
The
following factors consist of the system ... variables ... for which
[the Court] found scientific support that is generally accepted by
experts. Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 298-99.
37
T. Lineup Composition and Fillers Factors 1. Courts Findings
The Court found: The way that a live or photo lineup is constructed can also affect the reliability of an identification. Properly constructed lineups test a witness memory and decrease the chance that a witness is simply guessing. [ See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 251.] The Court listed the following features that can affect the construction of a fair lineup:
38
The
Court then stated, The record is unclear as to whether the use of
fillers that match a witness pre-lineup description is more reliable
than fillers that resemble an actual suspect (to the extent there is a
difference between the two). Id. at 252.
The Court advised that: [C]ourts should consider whether a lineup is poorly constructed when evaluating the admissibility of an identification. When appropriate, jurors should be told that poorly constructed or biased lineups can affect the reliability of an identification and enhance a witness confidence. [ Ibid.] 2. Committees Discussion The Committee did not make any changes to the language proposed by the subcommittee for these factors.
18 19
3. Recommended Paragraphs
The approved language states: (1) Lineup Composition: A suspect should not stand out from other members of the lineup. The reason is simple: an array of look-alikes forces witnesses to examine their memory. In addition, a biased lineup may inflate a witnesss confidence in the identification because the selection process seemed so easy to the witness.18 It is, of course, for you to determine whether the lineup was biased or not and whether the composition of the lineup had any affect on the reliability of the identification. (2) Fillers: Lineups should include a number of possible choices for the witness, commonly referred to as fillers. The greater the number of choices, the more likely the procedure will serve as a reliable test of the witnesss memory. A minimum of six persons or photos should be included in the lineup.19 Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 251. Ibid.
39
U. Multiple Viewings Factor 1. Courts Findings
The Court stated: Viewing a suspect more than once during an investigation can affect the reliability of the later identification. The problem, as the Special Master found, is that successive views of the same person can make it difficult to know whether the later identification stems from a memory of the original event or a memory of the earlier identification procedure. [ See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 255.] Additionally, the Court noted: It is typical for eyewitnesses to look through mugshot books in search of a suspect. Investigations may also involve multiple identification procedures. Based on the record, there is no impact on the reliability of the second identification procedure when a picture of the suspect was not present in photographs examined earlier. [ Ibid.] The Court further explained that Multiple identification procedures that involve more than one viewing of the same suspect, though, can create a risk of mugshot exposure and mug-shot commitment. Ibid.
40
The
Court concluded, Both mugshot exposure and mugshot commitment can
affect the reliability of the witness ultimate identification and create
a greater risk of misidentification. [ Id. at 256.]
2. Committees Discussion It was recognized by the Committee that while there is language in Henderson advising that mugshot exposure and mugshot commitment can affect the reliability of the identification, the word mugshot should not be stated to the jury. Therefore, the Committee revised the end of the footnote to state that the jury should be instructed on the concepts implicated by those terms without using the word mugshot. In addition, the Committee agreed to include a reference to the model jury charge, Identity-Police Photos.
3. Recommended Paragraph
The approved language states: (3) Multiple Viewing: When a witness views a person in multiple identification procedures, the witnesss memory of the actual perpetrator can be replaced by the witnesss memory of the person seen in the multiple procedures. In this way, when a witness views an innocent suspect in multiple identification procedures, the risk of mistaken identification is increased. You may consider whether the witness viewed the suspect multiple times and, if so, whether viewing the suspect in multiple procedures affected the reliability of the identification. 20
20
Henderson,
supra, 208 N.J. at 255-56. If either mugshot exposure (no
identification in first lineup/photo array, later identification of
someone in first procedure in second lineup/photo array) or mugshot
commitment (selection of person in lineup who was identified in previous
photo array) are part of the evidence, the jury should be instructed on
the concepts implicated by those terms without using the word mugshot.
See model charge on Identity-Police Photos.
41
V. Showups Factor
1. Courts Findings The Henderson Court explained: Showups are essentially single-person lineups: a single suspect is presented to a witness to make an identification. Showups often occur at the scene of a crime soon after its commission. [ Id. at 259.] Further, the Court stated: By their nature, showups are suggestive and cannot be performed blind or double-blind. Nonetheless, as the Special Master found, the risk of misidentification is not heightened if a showup is conducted immediately after the witnessed event, ideally within two hours because the benefits of a fresh memory seem to balance the risks of undue suggestion. [ Ibid.] The Court recognized that We have previously found showups to be inherently suggestive, see Herrera, supra, 187 N.J. at 504, 902 A.2d 177, and other states have limited the admissibility of showup identifications. Ibid. Therefore, the Court concluded: Thus, the record casts doubt on the reliability of showups conducted more than two hours after an event, which present a heightened risk of misidentification. As with lineups, showup administrators should instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the culprit and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification. That said, lineups are a preferred identification procedure because we continue to believe that showups, while sometimes necessary, are inherently suggestive. See Herrera, supra, 187 N.J. at 504, 902 A.2d 177. [ Id. at 261.]
42
2. Committees Discussion
The original paragraph reviewed by the Committee stated: Showups: In this case, the witness identified the defendant during a showup, that is, defendant was the only person shown to the witness at that time. Sometimes it is necessary for the police to conduct a showup, or one- on-one identification procedure, even though such a procedure is suggestive in nature. Although with immediate showups the benefits of a fresh memory may balance the risks of undue suggestion, showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk of misidentification. Also, police officers should instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification. If you find that the showup in this case was conducted more than two hours after the perpetrator was last seen by the witness, and/or if you find that the person conducting the lineup failed to instruct the witness that the person he/she was about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime, and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification, you may consider whether this/these factor(s) affected the accuracy of the identification. The subcommittee members explained that in crafting this paragraph they strived to balance the language in Henderson that showups are inherently suggestive and that there is a heightened risk of misidentification. They also pointed out that this paragraph should be charged in every case in which there is a showup procedure. The Committee had a lengthy discussion on this factor. One member was concerned with the proposed language, which in his opinion did not leave any room for whether the suggestiveness of the procedure should be considered within the first two hours. That member said that if the showup is outside two hours the jury can consider whether these factors weigh against the accuracy of
43
the
identification, and not just that it affected the accuracy of the
identification. Therefore, that member suggested that the end of this
paragraph state that You may consider whether these factors weigh
against the accuracy of the identification.
The Committee considered the Special Masters findings that the risk of misidentification is not heightened if a showup is conducted immediately after the witnessed event, ideally within two hours because the benefits of a fresh memory seem to balance the risks of undue suggestion. Id. at 259. Further, the Committee noted the statement in Henderson that the reliability of showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk of misidentification. Id. at 261. The Committee then expressed the importance of explaining the two hours difference to the jury: (1) if the showup is done within the first two hours and the witness is given the appropriate instructions this may balance the risks of an increased level of suggestiveness; (2) if the showup is done after two hours then there is a heightened risk of misidentification; and (3) that they must consider whether the identification was reliable or the result of an unduly suggestive procedure. A discussion then ensued on the use of immediate in the third sentence that Although with immediate showups the benefits of a fresh memory may balance the risks of undue suggestion, showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk of misidentification. It was expressed that Henderson does not define immediately but instead the Special Master referred to within two hours. The Committee could not reach a consensus on
44
this issue. Therefore, the Committee agreed to delete with immediate showups in that sentence.
The Committee also revised the next sentence stating Also, police officers should instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification to state must instruct and not should instruct. It was then asserted that the charge does not include any language on what the jury should consider when the identification is done properly. For example, the jury is not told how to consider a showup that was conducted within ten minutes, and the appropriate instructions were given. Some members were strongly opposed to including such an instruction in the charge. They argued that Henderson states, by their nature, showups are suggestive. Id. at 259. Further, those member noted the Special Masters statement that the benefits of fresh memory seem to balance the risks of undue suggestion was hardly a positive statement on this procedure. It was then expressed by one of the subcommittee members the importance of including the phrase Even though such a procedure is suggestive in nature to maintain the paragraphs balance. The Committee agreed to begin the second sentence with that phrase. The Committee then agreed to delete the last sentence and revise it to the following: In determining whether the identification is reliable or the result of undue suggestiveness procedure, you should consider how much time elapsed after the perpetrator was last seen by the
45
witness, and whether the appropriate instructions were given to the witness.
3. Recommended Paragraph The Committee approved the following: [CHARGE IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THERE IS A SHOWUP PROCEDURE] (4) Showups: In this case, the witness identified the defendant during a showup, that is, defendant was the only person shown to the witness at that time. Even though such a procedure is suggestive in nature, sometimes it is necessary for the police to conduct a showup, or one-on-one identification procedure. Although the benefits of a fresh memory may balance the risk of undue suggestion, showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk of misidentification. Also, police officers must instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification. In determining whether the identification is reliable or the result of undue suggestive procedure, you should consider how much time elapsed after the perpetrator was last seen by the witness, and whether the appropriate instructions were given to the witness. 21
21
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 259-61
46
W. Identification Lineup Procedure
1. Lineup Administrator Factor a. Courts Findings With regards to the lineup procedure, the Court stated, An identification may be unreliable if the lineup procedure is not administered in double-blind or blind fashion. See Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 248. Further, the Court distinguished the two methods of conducting the lineup: (1) Double-blind administrators do not know who the actual suspect is. Its purpose is to prevent an administrator from intentionally or unintentionally influencing a witness identification decision; (2) Blind administrators are aware of that information but shield themselves from knowing where the suspect is located in the lineup or photo array. [Ibid.] The Court further pointed out that Research has shown that lineup administrators familiar with the suspect may leak that information by consciously or unconsciously communicating to witnesses which lineup member is the suspect. Ibid. Moreover, the Court added: The consequences are clear: a non-blind lineup procedure can affect the reliability of a lineup because even the best- intentioned, non-blind administrator can act in a way that inadvertently sways an eyewitness trying to identify a suspect. An ideal lineup administrator, therefore, is someone who is not investigating the particular case and does not know who the suspect is. [ Id. at 249.]
47
The Court further stated:
The State understandably notes that police departments, no matter their size, have limited resources, and those limits can make it impractical to administer lineups double-blind in all cases. An alternative technique, which Dr. Wells referred to as the envelope method, helps address that challenge. It relies on single-blind administration: an officer who knows the suspects identity places single lineup photographs into different envelopes, shuffles them, and presents them to the witness. The officer/administrator then refrains from looking at the envelopes or pictures while the witness makes an identification. This blinding technique is cost-effective and can be used when resource constraints make it impractical to perform double-blind administration. [ Id. at 249-50.] The Court concluded that the failure to perform blind lineup procedures can increase the likelihood of misidentification. Id. at 250. b. Committees Discussion The subcommittee in crafting this paragraph agreed that this factor should be charged in every case where the police conduct an identification lineup procedure. In addition, they included an optional paragraph to address circumstances where the police do not use a blind administrator. The Committee approved the language as proposed by the subcommittee. c. Recommended Paragraph The approved language states:
48
[CHARGE (a) and (b) IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THE POLICE CONDUCT AN IDENTIFICATION LINEUP PROCEDURE]22
In determining the reliability of the identification, you should also consider whether the identification procedure was properly conducted.
(a) Double-blind:
A lineup administrator who knows which lineup member is the suspect may
intentionally or unintentionally convey this knowledge to the witness,
thereby increasing the chance that the witness will identify the suspect
even if the suspect is innocent. For this reason, whenever feasible,
lineups and photo-spreads should be conducted by an officer who does not
know the identity of the suspect.23
[CHARGE IF BLIND ADMINISTRATOR IS NOT USED] If a police officer who does not know the suspects identity is not available, then the officer should use a method that does not allow him/her to see the photos as the witness looks at them. In this case, it is alleged that the person who presented the lineup [did/did not] know the identity of the suspect. [In this case, it is alleged that a police officer who did not know the suspects identity was not available, and the police did/did not compensate for that by conducting a procedure where the officer did not see the photos as the witness looked at them.]
22
and estimator variables.) 23
To
help jurors weigh that evidence, they must be told about relevant
factors and their effect on reliability. Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at
219 (asking the Criminal Practice Committee and the Committee on Model
Criminal Jury Charges to draft proposed revisions to this charge and
address various system
Id. at 248-50.
49
2. InstructionsFactor
a. Courts Findings With regards to Pre-identification Instructions, the Court stated: Identification procedures should begin with instructions to the witness that the suspect may or may not be in the lineup or array and that the witness should not feel compelled to make an identification. There is a broad consensus for that conclusion. [ Id. at 250.] The Court found that the failure to give proper pre-lineup instructions can increase the risk of misidentification. Ibid. b. Committees Discussion Like the Lineup Administrator factor, the Committee agreed that this factor should also be charged in every case where the police conduct an identification lineup procedure.
24 25
The Committee approved the language as proposed by the subcommittee.
c. Recommended Paragraph The approved language states: You may consider this factor when you consider the circumstances under which the identification was made, and when you evaluate the overall reliability of the identification.24 (b) Instructions: You should consider what was or what was not said to the witness prior to viewing a photo array.25 Identification procedures should begin with instructions to the witness that the perpetrator may or may not be in the array and that the witness should not feel compelled to make an identification. The failure to give this instruction can increase the risk of misidentification. If you find that the police [did/did Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 248-50. See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995).
50
not] give this instruction to the witness, you may take this factor into account when evaluating the identification evidence.26
26
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 250.
51
3. Feedback Factor
a. Courts Findings The Court stated, Information received by witnesses both before and after an identification can affect their memory. Id. at 253. The Court explained: Confirmatory or post-identification feedback presents the same risks. It occurs when police signal to eyewitnesses that they correctly identified the suspect. That confirmation can reduce doubt and engender a false sense of confidence in a witness. Feedback can also falsely enhance a witness recollection of the quality of his or her view of an event. [ Ibid.]
Therefore,
the Court concluded, feedback affects the reliability identification in
that it can distort memory, create a false sense of confidence, and
alter a witness report of how he or she viewed an event. Id. at 255.
Moreover, based upon the record, the Court found: [N]on-State actors like co-witnesses and other sources of information can affect the independent nature and reliability of identification evidence and inflate witness confidence -- in the same way that law enforcement feedback can. As a result, law enforcement officers should instruct witnesses not to discuss the identification process with fellow witnesses or obtain information from other sources. [ Id. at 271.] b. Committees Discussion The only change the Committee made to the paragraph on Feedback proposed by the subcommittee was to revise the bracketed text above this factor to state [CHARGE IF FEEDBACK IS AN ISSUE IN THE CASE] rather than stating [CHARGE IF SUPPORTED BY THE EVIDENCE].
of an
52
c. Recommended Paragraph
The following was approved: [CHARGE IF FEEDBACK IS AN ISSUE IN THE CASE] (c) Feedback: Feedback occurs when police or other witnesses to an event signal to eyewitnesses that they correctly identified the suspect. That confirmation poses a risk of endangering a false sense of confidence in a witness. Feedback can also present a risk of falsely enhancing a witnesss recollection of the quality of his or her view of an event. It is for you to determine whether or not the recollection of the witness was affected by feedback or whether the recollection instead reflects the accurate perceptions of the witness during the event.27
27
Henderson,
supra, 208 N.J. at 253-55; see also Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (quoting
Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781 n.2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494
n.8 (Utah 1986)).
53
X. Concluding Paragraphs
1. Committees Discussion The Committee approved the final three paragraphs as proposed by the subcommittee. The first paragraph includes the language in the current charge in the last identification procedure factor on page 5. However, the last sentence was added to state Such information can affect the independent nature and reliability of a witnesss identification and inflate witness confidence in that identification. In addition the footnote now cites to State v. Chen, 208 N.J. 307(2011), instead of State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (2006). The first sentence of the next paragraph, except for some minor changes, is from factor 8 of the current charge. The next two sentences are new: Keep in mind that the presence of any single factor or combination of factor(s), however, is not an indication that a particular witness is incorrect. Instead, you may consider the factor or factor(s) which I have discussed as you assess all of the circumstances of the case, including all of the testimony and documentary evidence in determining whether a particular identification made by a witness is accurate and thus worthy of your consideration as you determine whether the State has met its burden to prove identification beyond a reasonable doubt. The subsequent sentences beginning with Unless the in-court and out-of- court identifications... to the end of the charge are from the current charge. 2. Recommended Paragraphs The Committee approved the following:
54
You
may consider whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions,
or identifications given by other witnesses, to photographs or
newspaper accounts, or to any other information or influence, that may
have affected the independence of his/her identification.28 Such
information can affect the independent nature and reliability of a
witnesss identification and inflate witness confidence in that
identification.
You are also free to consider any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case that you consider relevant to your determination whether the identifications were reliable. Keep in mind that the presence of any single factor or combination of factor(s), however, is not an indication that a particular witness is incorrect. Instead, you may consider the factor or factor(s) which I have discussed as you assess all of the circumstances of the case, including all of the testimony and documentary evidence in determining whether a particular identification made by a witness is accurate and thus worthy of your consideration as you determine whether the State has met its burden to prove identification beyond a reasonable doubt. Unless the in-court and out-of-court identifications resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the in-court and/or out-of-court identification procedures, they should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of an identification is for you to decide. If, after consideration of all of the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after consideration of all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
28
State v. Chen, 208 N.J. 307 (2011).
55
Respectfully Submitted,
Honorable Samuel D. Natal, Chair Honorable Greta Gooden-Brown, Vice-Chair Honorable Robert C. Billmeier Honorable Joseph C. Cassini, III Honorable Liliana S. DeAvila-Silebi Honorable Marianne Espinosa Honorable Thomas M. Moore Honorable Ronald L. Reisner Honorable Lisa Rose Honorable Francis B. Schultz Honorable Michael A. Toto Joseph E. Krakora, Public Defender Deborah Bartolomey, Esq. Robert J. Brass, Esq. Annmarie Cozzi, Esq. Catherine A. Foddai, Esq. Mark H. Friedman, Esq. Christine H. Kim, Esq. Richard S. Lehrich, Esq. John K. McNamara, Esq. Alison S. Perrone, Esq. Kevin G. Walsh, Esq. Jack L. Weinberg, Esq. Gwendolyn J. Williams, Esq. Michael J. Williams, Esq. Staff: Maria Pogue, Esq.
56
APPENDIX A PROPOSED CHARGES
Page 1 of 10 Revised 1/9/12
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS
(Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find this defendant guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed the crime. The defendant has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proven beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed it. The State has presented the testimony of [insert name of witness who identified defendant]. You will recall that this witness identified the defendant in court as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. The State also presented testimony that on a prior occasion before this trial, this witness identified the defendant as the person who committed this offense [these offenses]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the witnesss identification of the defendant is reliable and
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 2 of 10
believable, or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. Eyewitness identification evidence must be scrutinized carefully. Human beings do have the ability to recognize other people from past experiences and to identify them at a later time. But human memory is not foolproof. Research has shown that human memory is not at all like a video recording that a witness need only replay to remember what happened. Human memory is far more complex.2 I will instruct you on specific factors you should consider in this case in determining whether the eyewitness identification evidence is reliable. In evaluating this identification, you should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, the witnesss ability to make those observations and perceptions, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.3 In evaluating the identifications, you should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification were based, and the witnesss ability to make
1
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v.
Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981); State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119
(App. Div. 1996).
2 3
State v. Henderson, 208 N.J. 208, 245 (2011). State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007).
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 3 of 10
those observations and perceptions. If you determine that the out-of-court identification is not reliable, you may still consider the witnesss in-court identification of the defendant if you find it to be reliable. Unless the in-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the out-of-court identification procedure, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issues of the reliability of both the in-court and out-of-court identifications is for you to decide.4 To decide whether the identification testimony is sufficiently reliable evidence that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged, you should evaluate the testimony of the witness in light of the factors for considering credibility that I have already explained to you. In addition, you should consider the following factors that are related to the witness, the alleged perpetrator of the crime, and the criminal incident itself.5 In particular, you should consider [choose appropriate factors from one through six below]:
(1)
The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention:
In evaluating the reliability of the identification, you should assess
the witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at
the time of the offense and the witnesss degree of attention to the
perpetrator at the time of the offense. In making this assessment you
should consider the following [choose appropriate factors from (a) through (i) below]:
(a) Stress: Even under the best viewing conditions, high levels of stress can reduce an eyewitnesss ability to recall and make an accurate identification. Therefore, you should consider the level of the witnesss stress and whether
4
identification, and jurys credibility determinations).
United
States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 229-232, 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1933-1935, 1940
(manner in which lineup or other identification procedure conducted
relevant to reliability of out-of-court identification and in-court
identification following out-of-court
5
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 247.
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 4 of 10
that stress, if any, distracted the witness or made it harder for him or her to identify the perpetrator.6
(b) Weapons Focus: You should consider whether the witness saw the weapon during the incident. The presence of a weapon can distract the witness and take the witnesss attention away from the perpetrators face. As a result, the presence of a visible weapon may reduce the reliability of a subsequent identification if the crime is of a short duration. In considering this factor, you should take into account the duration of the crime because the longer the duration of the event, the more time the witness may have to adapt to the presence of the weapon and focus on other details.7 (c) Duration: The amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. Although there is no minimum time required to make an accurate identification a brief or fleeting contact is less likely to produce an accurate identification than a more prolonged exposure to the perpetrator. In addition, the time estimate given by a witness may not always be accurate because witnesses tend to think events lasted longer than they actually did.8 (d) Distance: A person is easier to recognize when close by. The greater the distance between an eyewitness and a perpetrator, the higher the risk of a mistaken identification. In addition, a witnesss estimate of how far he or she was from the perpetrator may not always be accurate because people tend to have difficulty estimating distances.9 (e) Lighting: Inadequate lighting can reduce the reliability of an identification. You should consider the lighting conditions that were present at the time of the alleged crime in this case.10 (f) Intoxication: An identification given by a witness with a high level of alcohol at the time of the incident tends to be more unreliable than an identification given by a witness who did not have a high level of alcohol. 11 (g) Disguises/Changed Appearance: The perpetrators use of a disguise can affect a witnesss ability to both remember and identify the perpetrator. Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 261-62. Id. at 262-63. Id. at 264. Ibid. Ibid. Id. at 265. Although Henderson only addressed alcohol if there is evidence of impairment by drugs or other substances the parties may want to have the jury consider this issue.
6 7 8 9 10 11
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 5 of 10
Disguises can be as simple as hats or sunglasses, as well as masks, and can reduce the accuracy of an identification.12 Similarly, if facial features are altered between the time of the event and a subsequent identification procedure, the accuracy of the identification may decrease.13 (2) Prior Description of Perpetrator: Another factor for your consideration is the accuracy of any description the witness gave before identifying the perpetrator. Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided details or was just general in nature, and whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator. (3) Confidence and Accuracy: You heard testimony that (insert name of witness) made a statement at the time of his/her identification of defendant from the photo array/line-up concerning his/her level of certainty that the photograph he/she selected is in fact the person who committed the crime. Although some research has found that highly confident witnesses are more likely to make accurate identifications, eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy.14 (4) Time Elapsed: Memories fade with time. As a result, delays between the commission of a crime and the time an identification is made can affect the reliability of the identification. In other words, the more time that passes, the greater the possibility that a witnesss memory of a perpetrator will weaken.15 (5) Cross-Racial Effects: The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.16 (6) Catch-all Factor: You may also consider the age of the identifying witness and any other relevant characteristics of the witness.
12 13 14 15 16
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 266. Ibid.
Id. at 253-55. Id. at 267.
This
instruction must be given whenever there is a cross-racial
identification. Id. at 299 (modifying State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112,
132 (1999)).
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 6 of 10
In evaluating the reliability of a witnesss identification, you should also consider the circumstances under which any out-of-court identification was made, and whether or not it was the result of a suggestive procedure, including everything done or said by law enforcement to the witness during the identification process. You should consider the following factors: [Charge if appropriate]:17 (1) Lineup Composition: A suspect should not stand out from other members of the lineup. The reason is simple: an array of look-alikes forces witnesses to examine their memory. In addition, a biased lineup may inflate a witnesss confidence in the identification because the selection process seemed so easy to the witness.18 It is, of course, for you to determine whether the lineup was biased or not and whether the composition of the lineup had any affect on the reliability of the identification. (2) Fillers: Lineups should include a number of possible choices for the witness, commonly referred to as fillers. The greater the number of choices, the more likely the procedure will serve as a reliable test of the witnesss memory. A minimum of six persons or photos should be included in the lineup.19 (3) Multiple Viewing: When a witness views a person in multiple identification procedures, the witnesss memory of the actual perpetrator can be replaced by the witnesss memory of the person seen in the multiple procedures. In this way, when a witness views an innocent suspect in multiple identification procedures, the risk of mistaken identification is increased. You may consider whether the witness viewed the suspect multiple times and, if so, whether viewing the suspect in multiple procedures affected the reliability of the identification. 20
17
The following factors consist of the system ... variables ... for which [the
Court] found scientific support that is generally accepted by experts. Henderson, supra,
208 N.J. at 298-99.
18 19 20
Id. at 251
Ibid. Id. at 255-56. If either mugshot exposure (no identification in first lineup/photo
array,
later identification of someone in first procedure in second
lineup/photo array) or mugshot commitment (selection of person in lineup
who was identified in previous photo array) are part of the evidence,
the jury should be instructed on the concepts implicated by those terms
without using the word mugshot. See model charge on Identity-Police
Photos.
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 7 of 10
[CHARGE IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THERE IS A SHOWUP PROCEDURE] (4) Showups: In this case, the witness identified the defendant during a showup, that is, defendant was the only person shown to the witness at that time. Even though such a procedure is suggestive in nature, sometimes it is necessary for the police to conduct a showup, or one-on-one identification procedure. Although the benefits of a fresh memory may balance the risk of undue suggestion, showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk of misidentification. Also, police officers must instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification. In determining whether the identification is reliable or the result of undue suggestive procedure, you should consider how much time elapsed after the perpetrator was last seen by the witness, and whether the appropriate instructions were given to the witness.21 [CHARGE (a) and (b) IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THE POLICE CONDUCT AN IDENTIFICATION LINEUP PROCEDURE]22 In determining the reliability of the identification, you should also consider whether the identification procedure was properly conducted.
(a) Double-blind:
A lineup administrator who knows which lineup member is the suspect may
intentionally or unintentionally convey this knowledge to the witness,
thereby increasing the chance that the witness will identify the suspect
even if the suspect is innocent. For this reason, whenever feasible,
lineups and photo-spreads should be conducted by an officer who does not
know the identity of the suspect.23
[CHARGE IF BLIND ADMINISTRATOR IS NOT USED] If a police officer who does not know the suspects identity is not available, then the officer should use a method that does not allow him/her to see the photos as the witness looks at them. In this case, it is alleged that the person who presented the lineup [did/did not] know the identity of the suspect. Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 259-61.
21 22
and address various system and estimator variables.) 23
To
help jurors weigh that evidence, they must be told about relevant
factors and their effect on reliability. Id. at 219 (asking the Criminal
Practice Committee and the Committee on Model Criminal Jury Charges to
draft proposed revisions to this charge
Id. at 248-50.
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 8 of 10
[In this case, it is alleged that a police officer who did not know the suspects identity was not available, and the police did/did not compensate for that by conducting a procedure where the officer did not see the photos as the witness looked at them.] [RESUME MAIN CHARGE] You may consider this factor when you consider the circumstances under which the identification was made, and when you evaluate the overall reliability of the identification.24 (b) Instructions: You should consider what was or what was not said to the witness prior to viewing a photo array.25 Identification procedures should begin with instructions to the witness that the perpetrator may or may not be in the array and that the witness should not feel compelled to make an identification. The failure to give this instruction can increase the risk of misidentification. If you find that the police [did/did not] give this instruction to the witness, you may take this factor into account when evaluating the identification evidence.26 [CHARGE IF FEEDBACK IS AN ISSUE IN THE CASE] (c) Feedback: Feedback occurs when police or other witnesses to an event signal to eyewitnesses that they correctly identified the suspect. That confirmation poses a risk of endangering a false sense of confidence in a witness. Feedback can also present a risk of falsely enhancing a witnesss recollection of the quality of his or her view of an event. It is for you to determine whether or not the recollection of the witness was affected by feedback or whether the recollection instead reflects the accurate perceptions of the witness during the event.27 [RESUME MAIN CHARGE] You may consider whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions, or identifications given by other witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any
24 25 26 27
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 248-50.
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995). Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 250. Id. at 253-55; see also Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at
781 n.2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n.8 (Utah 1986)).
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 9 of 10
other information or influence, that may have affected the independence of his/her identification.28 Such information can affect the independent nature and reliability of a witnesss identification and inflate witness confidence in that identification. You are also free to consider any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case that you consider relevant to your determination whether the identifications were reliable. Keep in mind that the presence of any single factor or combination of factor(s), however, is not an indication that a particular witness is incorrect. Instead, you may consider the factor or factor(s) which I have discussed as you assess all of the circumstances of the case, including all of the testimony and documentary evidence in determining whether a particular identification made by a witness is accurate and thus worthy of your consideration as you determine whether the State has met its burden to prove identification beyond a reasonable doubt. Unless the in- court and out-of-court identifications resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the in-court and/or out-of-court identification procedures, they should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of an identification is for you to decide. If, after consideration of all of the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after consideration of all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt
28
State v. Chen, 208 N.J. 307 (2011).
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 10 of 10
that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
Page 1 of 5 Revised 11/18/11
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
(Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find defendant guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this person is the person who committed the crime. (Defendant) has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proved each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proved beyond a reasonable doubt that (this defendant) is the person who committed it. The State has presented testimony of [insert name of witness who identified defendant]. You will recall that this witness identified the defendant as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the identification of (defendant) is reliable and believable, or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude that (this defendant) is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. 1
United
States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v.
Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981); State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super.
113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 2 of 5 Eyewitness identification evidence must be scrutinized carefully. Human beings do have the ability to recognize other people from past experiences and to identify them at a later time. But human memory is not foolproof. Research has shown that human memory is not at all like a video recording that a witness need only replay to remember what happened. Human memory is far more complex.2 You should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.3 In deciding what weight, if any, to give to the identification testimony, you should consider the following factors [cite appropriate factors]:
(1)
The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention:
In evaluating the reliability of the identification, you should assess
the witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at
the time of the offense and the witnesss degree of attention to the
perpetrator at the time of the offense. In making this assessment you
should consider the following [choose appropriate factors from (a) through (i) below]:
(a) Stress: Even under the best viewing conditions, high levels of stress can reduce an eyewitnesss ability to recall and make an accurate identification. Therefore, you should consider the level of the witnesss stress and whether that stress, if any, distracted the witness or made it harder for him or her to identify the perpetrator.4 State v. Henderson, 208 N.J. 208, 245 (2011). State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007). Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 261-62.
2 3 4
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 3 of 5
5 6 7 8 9
Id. at 265. Although Henderson only addressed alcohol, if there is evidence of
(b) Weapons Focus:
You should consider whether the witness saw the weapon during the
incident. The presence of a weapon can distract the witness and take the
witnesss attention away from the perpetrators face. As a result, the
presence of a visible weapon may reduce the reliability of a subsequent
identification if the crime is of a short duration. In considering this
factor, you should take into account the duration of the crime because
the longer the duration of the event, the more time the witness may have
to adapt to the presence of the weapon and focus on other details.5
(c) Duration: The amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. Although there is no minimum time required to make an accurate identification a brief or fleeting contact is less likely to produce an accurate identification than a more prolonged exposure to the perpetrator. In addition, the time estimate given by a witness may not always be accurate because witnesses tend to think events lasted longer than they actually did.6 (d) Distance: A person is easier to recognize when close by. The greater the distance between an eyewitness and a perpetrator, the higher the risk of a mistaken identification. In addition, a witnesss estimate of how far he or she was from the perpetrator may not always be accurate because people tend to have difficulty estimating distances.7 (e) Lighting: Inadequate lighting can reduce the reliability of an identification. You should consider the lighting conditions that were present at the time of the alleged crime in this case.8 (f) Intoxication: An identification given by a witness with a high level of alcohol at the time of the incident tends to be more unreliable than an identification given by a witness who did not have a high level of alcohol. 9 (g) Disguises/Changed Appearance: The perpetrators use of a disguise can affect a witnesss ability to both remember and identify the perpetrator. Disguises can be as simple as hats or sunglasses, as well as masks, and can
reduce the accuracy of an identification.10
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 262-63. Id. at 264. Ibid. Ibid.
Similarly, if facial features are
impairment by drugs or other substances the parties may want to have the jury consider
this issue. 10
Id. at 266.
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 4 of 5 altered between the time of the event and a subsequent identification procedure, the accuracy of the identification may decrease.11 (2) Prior Description of Perpetrator: Another factor for your consideration is the accuracy of any description the witness gave before identifying the perpetrator. Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided details or was just general in nature, and whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator. (3) Time Elapsed: Memories fade with time. As a result, delays between the commission of a crime and the time an identification is made can affect the reliability of the identification. In other words, the more time that passes, the greater the possibility that a witnesss memory of a perpetrator will weaken.12 (4) Cross-Racial Effects: The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.13 (5) Catch-all Factor: You may also consider the age of the identifying witness and any other relevant characteristics of the witness. You may consider whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions, or identifications given by other witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other information or influence, that may have affected the independence of his/her identification.14 Such information can affect the independent nature and reliability of a witnesss identification and inflate witness confidence in that identification. You are also free to consider any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case that you consider relevant to your determination whether the
11 12 13
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 266.
Id. at 267. This instruction must be given whenever there is a cross-racial identification. Id.
at 299 (modifying State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 132 (1999)).
14
State v. Chen, 208 N.J. 307 (2011).
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 5 of 5 identification was reliable. Keep in mind that the presence of any single factor or combination of factor(s), however, is not an indication that a particular witness is incorrect. Instead, you may consider the factor or factor(s) which I have discussed as you assess all of the circumstances of the case, including all of the testimony and documentary evidence in determining whether a particular identification made by a witness is accurate and thus worthy of your consideration as you determine whether the State has met its burden to prove identification beyond a reasonable doubt. Unless the in-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of the identification is for you to decide. If, after considering all of the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after considering all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
Page 1 of 8 Revised 1/9/12
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
(Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find (defendant) guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this person is the person who committed the crime. (Defendant) has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proved each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proved beyond a reasonable doubt that (this defendant) is the person who committed it. The State has presented testimony that on a prior occasion before this trial, [insert name of witness who identified defendant] identified (defendant) as the person who committed [insert the offenses charged]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the identification of (defendant) is reliable and believable or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence that (this defendant) is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. 1
United
States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v.
Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981); State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super.
113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 2 of 8 Eyewitness identification evidence must be scrutinized carefully. Human beings do have the ability to recognize other people from past experiences and to identify them at a later time. But human memory is not foolproof. Research has shown that human memory is not at all like a video recording that a witness need only replay to remember what happened. Human memory is far more complex.2 You should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of thereliabilityoftheidentification.3 In deciding what weight, if any, to give to the identification testimony, you should consider the following factors [cite appropriate factors]: (1) The Witnesss Opportunity to View and Degree of Attention: In evaluating the reliability of the identification, you should assess the witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense and the witnesss degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense. In making this assessment you should consider the following [choose appropriate factors from (a) through (i) below]:
2 3 4
(a) Stress:
Even under the best viewing conditions, high levels of stress can
reduce an eyewitnesss ability to recall and make an accurate
identification. Therefore, you should consider the level of the witnesss
stress and whether that stress, if any, distracted the witness or made
it harder for him or her to identify the perpetrator.4
State v. Henderson, 208 N.J. 208, 245 (2011). State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007). Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 261-62.
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 3 of 8
5 6 7 8 9
Id. at 265. Although Henderson only addressed alcohol, if there is evidence of
(b) Weapons Focus:
You should consider whether the witness saw the weapon during the
incident. The presence of a weapon can distract the witness and take the
witnesss attention away from the perpetrators face. As a result, the
presence of a visible weapon may reduce the reliability of a subsequent
identification if the crime is of a short duration. In considering this
factor, you should take into account the duration of the crime because
the longer the duration of the event, the more time the witness may have
to adapt to the presence of the weapon and focus on other details.5
(c) Duration: The amount of time an eyewitness has to observe an event may affect the reliability of an identification. Although there is no minimum time required to make an accurate identification a brief or fleeting contact is less likely to produce an accurate identification than a more prolonged exposure to the perpetrator. In addition, the time estimate given by a witness may not always be accurate because witnesses tend to think events lasted longer than they actually did.6 (d) Distance: A person is easier to recognize when close by. The greater the distance between an eyewitness and a perpetrator, the higher the risk of a mistaken identification. In addition, a witnesss estimate of how far he or she was from the perpetrator may not always be accurate because people tend to have difficulty estimating distances.7 (e) Lighting: Inadequate lighting can reduce the reliability of an identification. You should consider the lighting conditions that were present at the time of the alleged crime in this case.8 (f) Intoxication: An identification given by a witness with a high level of alcohol at the time of the incident tends to be more unreliable than an identification given by a witness who did not have a high level of alcohol. 9 (g) Disguises/Changed Appearance: The perpetrators use of a disguise can affect a witnesss ability to both remember and identify the perpetrator. Disguises can be as simple as hats or sunglasses, as well as masks, and can
reduce the accuracy of an identification.10
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 262-63. Id. at 264. Ibid. Ibid.
Similarly, if facial features are
impairment by drugs or other substances the parties may want to have the jury consider
this issue. 10
Id. at 266.
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 4 of 8 altered between the time of the event and a subsequent identification procedure, the accuracy of the identification may decrease.11 (2) Prior Description of Perpetrator: Another factor for your consideration is the accuracy of any description the witness gave before identifying the perpetrator. Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided details or was just general in nature, and whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator. (3) Confidence and Accuracy: You heard testimony that (insert name of witness) made a statement at the time of his/her identification of defendant from the photo array/line-up concerning his/her level of certainty that the photograph he/she selected is in fact the person who committed the crime. Although some research has found that highly confident witnesses are more likely to make accurate identifications, eyewitness confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy.12 (4) Time Elapsed: Memories fade with time. As a result, delays between the commission of a crime and the time an identification is made can affect the reliability of the identification. In other words, the more time that passes, the greater the possibility that a witnesss memory of a perpetrator will weaken.13 (5) Cross-Racial Effects: The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.14 (6) Catch-all Factor: You may also consider the age of the identifying witness and any other relevant characteristics of the witness. In evaluating the reliability of a witnesss identification, you should also consider the circumstances under which the out-of-court identification was made, and whether or not it was the result of a suggestive procedure, including everything done or said by law
11 12 13 14
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 266.
Id. at 253-55. Id. at 267. This instruction must be given whenever there is a cross-racial identification. Id.
at 299 (modifying State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 132 (1999)).
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 5 of 8 enforcement to the witness during the identification process. You should consider the following factors: [Charge if appropriate]:15 (1) Lineup Composition: A suspect should not stand out from other members of the lineup. The reason is simple: an array of look-alikes forces witnesses to examine their memory. In addition, a biased lineup may inflate a witnesss confidence in the identification because the selection process seemed so easy to the witness.16 It is, of course, for you to determine whether the lineup was biased or not and whether the composition of the lineup had any affect on the reliability of the identification. (2) Fillers: Lineups should include a number of possible choices for the witness, commonly referred to as fillers. The greater the number of choices, the more likely the procedure will serve as a reliable test of the witnesss memory. A minimum of six persons or photos should be included in the lineup.17 (3) Multiple Viewing: When a witness views a person in multiple identification procedures, the witnesss memory of the actual perpetrator can be replaced by the witnesss memory of the person seen in the multiple procedures. In this way, when a witness views an innocent suspect in multiple identification procedures, the risk of mistaken identification is increased. You may consider whether the witness viewed the suspect multiple times and, if so, whether viewing the suspect in multiple procedures affected the reliability of the identification. 18 [CHARGE IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THERE IS A SHOWUP PROCEDURE] (4) Showups: In this case, the witness identified the defendant during a showup, that is, defendant was the only person shown to the witness at that time. Even though such a procedure is suggestive in nature, sometimes it is necessary for the police to conduct a showup, or one-on-one, identification procedure. Although the benefits of a fresh memory may balance the risks of undue suggestion, showups conducted more than two hours after an event present a heightened risk
15
The followingfactorsconsistofthesystem...variables...forwhich[theCourt]
found scientific support that is generally accepted by experts. Henderson, supra, 208
N.J. at 298-99.
16 17 18
Id. at 251.
Ibid. Id. at 255-56. If either mugshot exposure (no identification in first lineup/photo
array,
later identification of someone in first procedure in second
lineup/photo array) or mugshot commitment (selection of person in lineup
who was identified in previous photo array) are part of the evidence,
the jury should be instructed on the concepts implicated by those terms
without using the word mugshot. See model charge on Identity-Police
Photos.
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 6 of 8 of misidentification. Also, police officers must instruct witnesses that the person they are about to view may or may not be the person who committed the crime and that they should not feel compelled to make an identification. In determining whether the identification is reliable or the result of undue suggestive procedure, you should consider how much time elapsed after the perpetrator was last seen by the witness, and whether the appropriate instructions were given to the witness.19 [CHARGE (a) AND (b) IN EVERY CASE IN WHICH THE POLICE CONDUCT AN IDENTIFICATION LINEUP PROCEDURE]20 In determining the reliability of the identification, you should also consider whether the identification procedure was properly conducted.
(a) Double-blind:
A lineup administrator who knows which lineup member is the suspect may
intentionally or unintentionally convey this knowledge to the witness,
thereby increasing the chance that the witness will identify the suspect
even if the suspect is innocent. For this reason, whenever feasible,
lineups and photo-spreads should be conducted by an officer who does not
know the identity of the suspect.21
[CHARGE IF BLIND ADMINISTRATOR IS NOT USED] If a police officer who does not know the suspects identity is not available, then the officer should use a method that does not allow him/her to see the photos as the witness looks at them. In this case, it is alleged that the person who presented the lineup [did/did not] know the identity of the suspect. [In this case, it is alleged that a police officer who did not know the suspects identity was not available, and the police did/did not compensate for that by conducting a procedure where the officer did not see the photos as the witness looked at them.] [RESUME MAIN CHARGE] Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 259-61.
19 20
and address various system and estimator variables.) 21
To
help jurors weigh that evidence, they must be told about relevant
factors and their effect on reliability. Id. at 219 (asking the Criminal
Practice Committee and the Committee on Model Criminal Jury Charges to
draft proposed revisions to this charge
Id. at 248-50.
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 7 of 8 You may consider this factor when you consider the circumstances under which the identification was made, and when you evaluate the overall reliability of the identification.22 (b) Instructions: You should consider what was or what was not said to the witness prior to viewing a photo array.23 Identification procedures should begin with instructions to the witness that the perpetrator may or may not be in the array and that the witness should not feel compelled to make an identification. The failure to give this instruction can increase the risk of misidentification. If you find that the police [did/did not] give this instruction to the witness, you may take this factor into account when evaluating the identification evidence.24 [CHARGE IF FEEDBACK IS AN ISSUE IN THE CASE] (c) Feedback: Feedback occurs when police or other witnesses to an event signal to eyewitnesses that they correctly identified the suspect. That confirmation poses a risk of endangering a false sense of confidence in a witness. Feedback can also present a risk of falsely enhancing a witnesss recollection of the quality of his or her view of an event. It is for you to determine whether or not the recollection of the witness was affected by feedback or whether the recollection instead reflects the accurate perceptions of the witness during the event.25 [RESUME MAIN CHARGE] You may consider whether the witness was exposed to opinions, descriptions, or identifications given by other witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other information or influence, that may have affected the independence of his/her
22 23 24 25
Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 248-50.
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995). Henderson, supra, 208 N.J. at 250. Id. at 253-55; see also Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at
781 n.2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n.8 (Utah 1986)).
IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 8 of 8 identification.26 Such information can affect the independent nature and reliability of a witnesss identification and inflate witness confidence in that identification. You are also free to consider any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case that you consider relevant to your determination whether the identification was reliable. Keep in mind that the presence of any single factor or combination of factor(s), however, is not an indication that a particular witness is incorrect. Instead, you may consider the factor or factor(s) which I have discussed as you assess all of the circumstances of the case, including all of the testimony and documentary evidence in determining whether a particular identification made by a witness is accurate and thus worthy of your consideration as you determine whether the State has met its burden to prove identification beyond a reasonable doubt. Unless the out-of-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the out-of-court identification procedure, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of the identification is for you to decide. If, after considering all of the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after consideration of all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
26
State v. Chen, 208 N.J. 307 (2011).
APPENDIX B CURRENT CHARGES
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS
(Defendant) as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find this defendant guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed the crime. The defendant has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proved each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proved beyond a reasonable doubt that this defendant is the person who committed it. The State has presented the testimony of [insert name of witness who identified defendant]. You will recall that this witness identified the defendant in court as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. The State also presented testimony that on a prior occasion before this trial, this witness identified the defendant as the person who committed this offense [these offenses]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the witness identification of the defendant is reliable and believable, or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v. Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-
Revised 6/4/07
1
293 (1981); State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 2 of 6
to conclude that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. You should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence, standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.2 In evaluating the identifications, you should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identifications were based, and the witness ability to make those observations and perceptions. If you determine that the out-of-court identification is not reliable, you may still consider the witness in-court identification of the defendant if you find it to be reliable. Unless the in-court identification resulted from the witness observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the out-of-court identification procedure, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issues of the trustworthiness of both the in-court and out-of-court identifications are for you to decide.3 To decide whether the identification testimony is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude that this defendant is the person who committed the offense[s] charged, you should evaluate the testimony of the witness in light of the factors for considering credibility that I have already explained to you. In addition, you may consider the following factors [cite appropriate factors]:4
2 3
of-court identification, and jurys credibility determinations). 4
State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007).
United
States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 229-232, 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1933-1935, 1940
(manner in which lineup or other identification procedure conducted
relevant to reliability of out-of-court identification and in-court
identification following out-
The
first five factors listed below were enumerated in Neil v. Biggers, 409
U.S. 188, 199, 93 S.Ct. 375, 382 (1972), and United States v. Wade, 388
U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940, as the factors to be considered in
evaluating the likelihood of misidentification. New Jersey courts employ
the same analysis. State v. Madison, 109 N.J. 223, 239-240 (1988). See
also State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503, 520 (App. Div. 1995).
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 3 of 6
[If necessary or appropriate for purposes of clarity, the judge may comment on any evidence relevant to any of the following factors]5
5
(1) The witness opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense.6
(2) The witness degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense.7 (3) The accuracy of any description the witness gave prior to identifying the perpetrator.8 (4) The degree of certainty expressed by the witness in making any identification.9 See State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 128 (1999) (when identification is a critical issue in the case, the trial court is
obligated
to give the jury discrete and specific instruction that provides
appropriate guidelines to focus the jurys attention on how
to analyze and consider the trustworthiness of eyewitness identification); State v. Green, 86 N.J. at 292, 293 (noting that model charge could have been used as a guide, court holds that the defendant had a right to expect that the appropriate guidelines would be given, focusing the jurys attention on how to analyze and consider the factual issues with regard to the trustworthiness of [the witnesss] in-court identification); but see State v. Robinson, 165 N.J. 32, 42-45 (2000) (reaffirming obligation under Green to explain abstract identification factors in factual context of case, but holding that court need not necessarily summarize weaknesses of States evidence); see generally, State v. Gartland, 149 N.J. 456, 475 (1997) (holding that jury charges must relate the law to the specific facts in a case); State v. A. Gross, 121 N.J. 1 (1990) (same); State v. Concepcion, 111 N.J. 373 (1988) (same). 6 be appropriate to add the following to factor (1): . . . including the nature of the event being observed and the likelihood that the witness would perceive, remember, and relate it correctly. State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (2006) (quoting State v. Ramirez, 817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)).
Facts
that may be relevant to this factor include the witnesss ability to
observe what he/she said he/she saw, the amount of time during which the
witness saw the perpetrator, the distance from which the witness saw
the perpetrator, and the lighting conditions at the time. See Manson v.
Brathwaite, 432 U.S. 98, 114, 97 S.Ct. 2243, 2253 (1977); Neil v.
Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200-201, 93 S.Ct. at 382; State v. Madison, 109
N.J. at 239.
Where supported by evidence that the victim might have difficulty perceiving, recalling, or relating the events, it may
7
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether the witness was merely a passing or casual observer or one
who
would be expected to pay scrupulous attention to detail, whether the
witness was involved in a direct confrontation with the
perpetrator, whether the witness was nervous, shocked or scared as a result of any confrontation with the perpetrator, and whether the witnesss attention was focused on or away from the perpetrators features. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93 S.Ct. at 382-383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240.
8
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after
observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided
details or was just general in nature, whether the witness testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v.Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93 S.Ct. at 383; United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240-241; State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113 (App. Div. 1996). 9 Other relevant facts include whether, at a time prior to making the identification of this defendant, the witness either failed to identify the defendant or identified another person as the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at 383; Foster v. California, 394 U.S. 440, 442-443 & n.2, 89 S.Ct. 1127,
Facts
that may be relevant to this factor include whether witnesses making
the identification received inadvertent or intentional confirmation,
whether certainty was expressed at the time of the identification or
some time later, whether intervening events following the identification
affected the witnesss certainty, and whether the identification was
made spontaneously and remained consistent thereafter. See N.J. Attorney
General Guidelines for Preparing and Conducting Photo and Live Lineup
Identification Procedures, April 18, 2001, at 2 (quoted in Herrera, 187
N.J. at 190); National Institute of Justice, Convicted by Juries,
Exonerated by Science, June 1996, at 24 (available at
https://www.ndjrs.gov/pdffiles/dnaevid.pdf); Gary Wells & Amy
Bradfield, Good, You Identified the Suspect, 83 J. Applied Psychol. 360
(1998); Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781. Whether the witness made an
identification quickly upon viewing the suspect, or whether the witness
hesitated, may also be a relevant fact. See S. Sporer, Eyewitness
Identification Accuracy, Confidence, and Decision Times in Simultaneous
and Sequential Lineups, 78 J. Applied Psychol. 22, 23 (1993).
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 4 of 6
(5) The length of time between the witness observation of the offense and the first identification.10
10
383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 242.
See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115-116, 97 S.Ct. at
2253-2254; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at Facts that may
be relevant to this factor include whether the witness identification
at trial was different from, or the
11
same as, any prior identification that took place out-of-court. See State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. at 118.
12
Refer to the New Jersey Attorney General Guidelines, footnote 8 supra. The court should focus on any allegations of
suggestive words or conduct by law enforcement or other persons that may effect the suggestiveness of the identification
procedures.
13
See State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493 (2006), in which the New Jersey Supreme Court addressed the propriety of a show-
up identification; the majority opinion concluded that, while such a procedure is inherently suggestive, the identification
procedure employed there was reliable and did not result in a substantial likelihood of misidentification.
14 15 16
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995). Id.
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 233, 87 S.Ct. at 1935.
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 5 of 6
17 18 19 20 21 22
other
witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other
information or influence that may have affected the independence of
his/her identification.23
[CHARGE IN ALL CASES:] (8) Any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case which you consider relevant to your determination whether the identifications were reliable. [(9) Jury should be charged on any other relevant factor present in the case24] Id. Id. Id. Id., 87 S.Ct. at 1935-1936. See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting State v. Ramirez, 817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)). See N.J. Attorney General Guidelines, supra, Guideline I.B. (requiring administrator to instruct witness that perpetrator
may not be present); State v. Ledbetter, 881 A.2d 290 (Ct. 2005) (requiring jury instruction to that effect).
23 24
See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781 n. 2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n. 8 (Utah 1986)).
The
list of factors enumerated in Biggers and Madison is not exhaustive.
See State v. White, 158 N.J. 230, (1999) (in declining to find plain
error in identification charge, court notes that instruction went beyond
model charge, noting the discrepancy ... between identifications made
by different witnesses). Additional relevant factors that should be
brought to jurys attention include the witnesss inability to make an
in-court identification if asked to do so while on the witness stand,
any failure on the part of the State to record a line-up or preserve a
photo array, as bearing upon the probative value of the out-of-court
identification, see State v. Delgado, 188 N.J. 48, 63 (2006); State
v. Earle, 60 N.J. 550, 552 (1972); State v. Peterkin, 226 N.J. Super.
25, 46 (App. Div. 1988), and any discrepancies between identifications
made by different witnesses, State v. White, 158 N.J. 230, 248.
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT AND OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATIONS Page 6 of 6
[IN THE APPROPRIATE CASE,25 CHARGE THE FOLLOWING FACTOR:] (10) The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.26 [CHARGE IN ALL CASES:] Unless the in-court and out-of-court identifications resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the in-court and/or out-of-court identification procedures, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of the identification is for you to decide. If, after consideration of all of the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after consideration of all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
25
An instruction that cross-racial identification is a factor to be considered should be given only when ... identification is
a critical issue in the case, and an eyewitnesss cross-racial identification is not corroborated by other evidence giving it
independent reliability. State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. at 132; see also State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007) 26
Cromedy
holds that in order for the jury to determine the reliability of a
cross-racial identification not corroborated by independent evidence,
the jury must be informed of the potential risks associated with such
identifications, that the jury must be instructed about the possible
significance of the cross-racial identification factor.... 158 N.J. at
132-133. In State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007), the New Jersey Supreme
Court refused to rule that cross-ethnic charges were required in cases
involving an individuals identification of a person of another ethnic
background.
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v. Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981);
Revised 6/4/07
(Defendant),
as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State
has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a
reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged
offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed
the crime is upon the State. For you to find (defendant) guilty, the
State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this person is the
person who committed the crime. (Defendant) has neither the burden nor
the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone
else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine,
therefore, not only whether the State has proved each and every element
of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the
State has proved beyond a reasonable doubt that (this defendant) is the
person who committed it.
The State has presented testimony of [insert name of witness who identified defendant]. You will recall that this witness identified the defendant as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the identification of (defendant) is reliable and believable or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude that (this defendant) is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. You should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence,
1
State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 2 of 5 standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.2 In deciding what weight, if any, to give to the identification testimony, you may consider the following factors [cite appropriate factors]:3 [If necessary or appropriate for purposes of clarity, the judge may comment on any evidence relevant to any of the following factors]4
2 3
(1) The witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense.5
(2) The witnesss degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense.6 (3) The accuracy of any description the witness gave prior to identifying the perpetrator.7 (4) The degree of certainty expressed by the witness in making the identification.8 State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007). The first five factors listed below were enumerated in Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. 188, 199, 93 S.Ct. 375, 382 (1972), and
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940, as the factors to be considered in evaluating the likelihood of
misidentification. New Jersey courts employ the same analysis. State v. Madison, 109 N.J. 223, 239-240 (1988). See also State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503, 520 (App. Div. 1995).
4
See State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 128 (1999) (when identification is a critical issue in the case, the trial court is
obligated
to give the jury discrete and specific instruction that provides
appropriate guidelines to focus the jurys attention on how
to analyze and consider the trustworthiness of eyewitness identification); State v. Green, 86 N.J. at 292, 293 (noting that model charge could have been used as a guide, court holds that the defendant had a right to expect that the appropriate guidelines would be given, focusing the jurys attention on how to analyze and consider the factual issues with regard to the trustworthiness of [the witnesss] in-court identification); but see State v. Robinson, 165 N.J. 32, 42-45 (2000) (reaffirming obligation under Green to explain abstract identification factors in factual context of case, but holding that court need not necessarily summarize weaknesses of States evidence); see generally, State v. Gartland, 149 N.J. 456, 475 (1997) (holding that jury charges must relate the law to the specific facts in a case); State v. A. Gross, 121 N.J. 1 (1990) (same); State v. Concepcion, 111 N.J. 373 (1988) (same). 5 be appropriate to add the following to factor (1): . . . including the nature of the event being observed and the likelihood that the witness would perceive, remember, and relate it correctly. State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (2006) (quoting State v. Ramirez, 817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)).
Facts
that may be relevant to this factor include the witnesss ability to
observe what he/she said he/she saw, the amount of time during which the
witness saw the perpetrator, the distance from which the witness saw
the perpetrator, and the lighting conditions at the time. See Manson v.
Brathwaite, 432 U.S. 98, 114, 97 S.Ct. 2243, 2253 (1977); Neil v.
Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200-201, 93 S.Ct. at 382; State v. Madison, 109
N.J. at 239.
Where supported by evidence that the victim might have difficulty perceiving, recalling, or relating the events, it may
6
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether the witness was merely a passing or casual observer or one
who
would be expected to pay scrupulous attention to detail, whether the
witness was involved in a direct confrontation with the
perpetrator, whether the witness was nervous, shocked or scared as a result of any confrontation with the perpetrator, and whether the witnesss attention was focused on or away from the perpetrators features. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93 S.Ct. at 382-383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240.
7
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after
observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided
details or was just general in nature, whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v.Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93 S.Ct. at 383; United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240-241; State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113 (App. Div. 1996). 8
Facts
that may be relevant to this factor include whether witnesses making
the identification received inadvertent or intentional confirmation,
whether certainty was expressed at the time of the identification or
some time later, whether intervening events following the identification
affected the witnesss certainty, and whether the identification was
made spontaneously and
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 3 of 5 (5) The length of time between the witnesss observation of the perpetrator during the offense and the identification.9 (6) The circumstances under which the identification was made, and whether or not it was the product of a suggestive procedure10, including everything done or said by law enforcement to the witness before, during, or after the identification process.11 In making this determination you may consider the following circumstances: [REFER TO CIRCUMSTANCES OF THE IDENTIFICATION PROCEDURE AS NECESSARY FOR CLARITY, CHOOSING AS APPROPRIATE ANY OF THE FOLLOWING FACTORS, OR ANY OTHER FACTORS RELATING TO SUGGESTIVENESS, THAT ARE SUPPORTED BY THE EVIDENCE:] whether anything was said to the witness prior to viewing a photo array, line-up or showup;12 whether a photo array shown to the witness contained multiple photographs of the defendant;13 remained consistent thereafter. See N.J. Attorney General Guidelines for Preparing and Conducting Photo and Live Lineup Identification Procedures, April 18, 2001, at 2 (quoted in Herrera, 187 N.J. at 190); National Institute of Justice, Convicted by Juries, Exonerated by Science, June 1996, at 24 (available at https://www.ndjrs.gov/pdffiles/dnaevid.pdf); Gary Wells & Amy Bradfield, Good, You Identified the Suspect, 83 J. Applied Psychol. 360 (1998); Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781. Whether the witness made an identification quickly upon viewing the suspect, or whether the witness hesitated, may also be a relevant fact. See S. Sporer, Eyewitness Identification Accuracy, Confidence, and Decision Times in Simultaneous and Sequential Lineups, 78 J. Applied Psychol. 22, 23 (1993). Other relevant facts include whether, at a time prior to making the identification of this defendant, the witness either failed to identify the defendant or identified another person as the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at 383; Foster v. California, 394 U.S. 440, 442-443 & n.2, 89 S.Ct. 1127, 1128-1129 & n.2 (1969); United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 241. Madison cautions, with respect to an identification witnesss demonstrated certainty in his testimony, that a witnesss feeling of confidence in the details of memory generally do not validly measure the accuracy of the recollection, and that [i]n fact, witnesses frequently become more confident of the correctness of their memory over time while the actual memory trace is probably decaying. Id. at 241-242 (quoting W.LaFave and J.Israel, Criminal Procedure).
9
383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 242.
See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115-116, 97 S.Ct. at 2253-2254; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at
10
Refer to the New Jersey Attorney General Guidelines, footnote 8 supra. The court should focus on any allegations of
suggestive words or conduct by law enforcement or other persons that may effect the suggestiveness of the identification
procedures.
11
See State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493 (2006), in which the New Jersey Supreme Court addressed the propriety of a show-
up identification; the majority opinion concluded that, while such a procedure is inherently suggestive, the identification
procedure employed there was reliable and did not result in a substantial likelihood of misidentification.
12 13
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995). Id.
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 4 of 5
14 15 16 17 18 19 20
other
witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other
information or influence that may have affected the independence of
his/her identification.21
[CHARGE IN ALL CASES:] (7) Any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case which you consider relevant to your determination of whether the out-of-court identification was reliable. [(8) Jury should be charged on any other relevant factor present in the case22] United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 233, 87 S.Ct. at 1935. Id. Id. Id. Id., 87 S.Ct. at 1935-1936. See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting State v. Ramirez, 817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)). See N.J. Attorney General Guidelines, supra, Guideline I.B. (requiring administrator to instruct witness that perpetrator
may not be present); State v. Ledbetter, 881 A.2d 290 (Ct. 2005) (requiring jury instruction to that effect).
21 22
See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781 n. 2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n. 8 (Utah 1986)).
The
list of factors enumerated in Biggers and Madison is not exhaustive.
See State v. White, 158 N.J. 230, (1999) (in declining to find plain
error in identification charge, court notes that instruction went beyond
model charge, noting the discrepancy ... between identifications made
by different witnesses). Additional relevant factors that should be
brought to jurys attention include the witnesss inability to make an
in-court identification if asked to do so while on the witness stand,
any failure on the part of the State to record a line-up or preserve a
photo array, as bearing upon the probative value of the out-of-court
identification, see State v. Delgado, 188 N.J. 48, 63 (2006); State
v. Earle, 60 N.J. 550, 552 (1972); State v. Peterkin, 226 N.J. Super.
25, 46 (App. Div. 1988), and any discrepancies between identifications
made by different witnesses, State v. White, 158
IDENTIFICATION: IN-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 5 of 5 [IN THE APPROPRIATE CASE,23 CHARGE THE FOLLOWING FACTOR:] (9) The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.24 [CHARGE IN ALL CASES:] Unless the in-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of the identification is for you to decide. If, after considering all the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after considering all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
N.J. 230, 248.
23
An instruction that cross-racial identification is a factor to be considered should be given only when ... identification is
a critical issue in the case, and an eyewitnesss cross-racial identification is not corroborated by other evidence giving it
independent reliability. State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. at 132; see also State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007). 24
Cromedy
holds that in order for the jury to determine the reliability of a
cross-racial identification not corroborated by independent evidence,
the jury must be informed of the potential risks associated with such
identifications, that the jury must be instructed about the possible
significance of the cross-racial identification factor.... 158 N.J. at
132-33. In State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007), the New Jersey Supreme
Court refused to rule that cross-ethnic charges were required in cases
involving an individuals identification of a person of another ethnic
background.
Revised 6/4/07 IDENTIFICATION: OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
(Defendant), as part of [his/her] general denial of guilt, contends that the State has not presented sufficient reliable evidence to establish beyond a reasonable doubt that [he/she] is the person who committed the alleged offense. The burden of proving the identity of the person who committed the crime is upon the State. For you to find (defendant) guilty, the State must prove beyond a reasonable doubt that this person is the person who committed the crime. (Defendant) has neither the burden nor the duty to show that the crime, if committed, was committed by someone else, or to prove the identity of that other person. You must determine, therefore, not only whether the State has proved each and every element of the offense charged beyond a reasonable doubt, but also whether the State has proved beyond a reasonable doubt that (this defendant) is the person who committed it. The State has presented testimony that on a prior occasion before this trial, [insert name of witness who identified defendant] identified (defendant) as the person who committed [insert the offense(s) charged]. According to the witness, [his/her] identification of the defendant was based upon the observations and perceptions that [he/she] made of the perpetrator at the time the offense was being committed. It is your function to determine whether the identification of (defendant) is reliable and believable or whether it is based on a mistake or for any reason is not worthy of belief.1 You must decide whether it is sufficiently reliable evidence upon which to conclude that (this defendant) is the person who committed the offense[s] charged. You should consider the observations and perceptions on which the identification was based, and the circumstances under which the identification was made. Although nothing may appear more convincing than a witnesss categorical identification of a perpetrator, you must critically analyze such testimony. Such identifications, even if made in good faith, may be mistaken. Therefore, when analyzing such testimony, be advised that a witnesss level of confidence,
1
State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113, 118-119 (App. Div. 1996).
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 228, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 1933 (1967); State v. Green, 86 N.J. 281, 291-293 (1981);
IDENTIFICATION:OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 2 of 5 standing alone, may not be an indication of the reliability of the identification.2 In deciding what weight, if any, to give to the identification testimony, you may consider the following factors [cite appropriate factors]:3 [If necessary or appropriate for purposes of clarity, the judge may comment on any evidence relevant to any of the following factors]4
2
3
(1) The witnesss opportunity to view the person who committed the offense at the time of the offense.5
(2) The witnesss degree of attention to the perpetrator at the time of the offense.6 (3) The accuracy of any description the witness gave prior to identifying the perpetrator.7 (4) The degree of certainty expressed by the witness in making the identification.8 State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59, 76 (2007). The first five factors listed below were enumerated in Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. 188, 199, 93 S.Ct. 375, 382 (1972), and
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940, as the factors to be considered in evaluating the likelihood of
misidentification. New Jersey courts employ the same analysis. State v. Madison, 109 N.J. 223, 239-240 (1988). See also State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503, 520 (App. Div. 1995).
4
See State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. 112, 128 (1999) (when identification is a critical issue in the case, the trial court is
obligated
to give the jury discrete and specific instruction that provides
appropriate guidelines to focus the jurys attention on how
to analyze and consider the trustworthiness of eyewitness identification); State v. Green, 86 N.J. at 292, 293 (noting that model charge could have been used as a guide, court holds that the defendant had a right to expect that the appropriate guidelines would be given, focusing the jurys attention on how to analyze and consider the factual issues with regard to the trustworthiness of [the witnesss] in-court identification); but see State v. Robinson, 165 N.J. 32, 42-45 (2000) (reaffirming obligation under Green to explain abstract identification factors in factual context of case, but holding that court need not necessarily summarize weaknesses of States evidence); see generally, State v. Gartland, 149 N.J. 456, 475 (1997) (holding that jury charges must relate the law to the specific facts in a case); State v. A. Gross, 121 N.J. 1 (1990) (same); State v. Concepcion, 111 N.J. 373 (1988) (same). 5 be appropriate to add the following to factor (1): . . . including the nature of the event being observed and the likelihood that the witness would perceive, remember, and relate it correctly. State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493, 509 (2006) (quoting State v. Ramirez, 817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)).
Facts
that may be relevant to this factor include the witnesss ability to
observe what he/she said he/she saw, the amount of time during which the
witness saw the perpetrator, the distance from which the witness saw
the perpetrator, and the lighting conditions at the time. See Manson v.
Brathwaite, 432 U.S. 98, 114, 97 S.Ct. 2243, 2253 (1977); Neil v.
Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200-201, 93 S.Ct. at 382; State v. Madison, 109
N.J. at 239.
Where supported by evidence that the victim might have difficulty perceiving, recalling, or relating the events, it may
6
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether the witness was merely a passing or casual observer or one
who
would be expected to pay scrupulous attention to detail, whether the
witness was involved in a direct confrontation with the
perpetrator, whether the witness was nervous, shocked or scared as a result of any confrontation with the perpetrator, and whether the witnesss attention was focused on or away from the perpetrators features. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93 S.Ct. at 382-383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240.
7
Facts that may be relevant to this factor include whether any description the witness gave of the perpetrator after
observing the incident but before making the identification was accurate or inaccurate, whether the prior description provided
details or was just general in nature, whether the witnesss testimony at trial was consistent with, or different from, his/her prior description of the perpetrator. See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115, 97 S.Ct. at 2253; Neil v.Biggers, 409 U.S. at 200, 93 S.Ct. at 383; United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 241, 87 S.Ct. at 1940; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 240-241; State v. Edmonds, 293 N.J. Super. 113 (App. Div. 1996). 8
Facts
that may be relevant to this factor include whether witnesses making
the identification received inadvertent or intentional confirmation,
whether certainty was expressed at the time of the identification or
some time later, whether intervening events following the identification
affected the witnesss certainty, and whether the identification was
made spontaneously and remained consistent thereafter. See N.J. Attorney
General Guidelines for Preparing and Conducting Photo and Live Lineup
IDENTIFICATION:OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 3 of 5 (5) The length of time between the witnesss observation of the perpetrator during the offense and the identification.9 (6) The circumstances under which the identification was made, and whether or not it was the product of a suggestive procedure10, including everything done or said by law enforcement to the witness before, during, or after the identification process.11 In making this determination you may consider the following circumstances: [REFER TO CIRCUMSTANCES OF THE IDENTIFICATION PROCEDURE AS NECESSARY FOR CLARITY, CHOOSING AS APPROPRIATE ANY OF THE FOLLOWING FACTORS, OR ANY OTHER FACTORS RELATING TO SUGGESTIVENESS, THAT ARE SUPPORTED BY THE EVIDENCE:]
9
383; State v. Madison, 109 N.J. at 242.
See Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. at 115-116, 97 S.Ct. at 2253-2254; Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. at 201, 93 S.Ct. at
10
Refer to the New Jersey Attorney General Guidelines, footnote 8 supra. The court should focus on any allegations of
suggestive words or conduct by law enforcement or other persons that may effect the suggestiveness of the identification
procedures.
11
See State v. Herrera, 187 N.J. 493 (2006), in which the New Jersey Supreme Court addressed the propriety of a show-
up identification; the majority opinion concluded that, while such a procedure is inherently suggestive, the identification
procedure employed there was reliable and did not result in a substantial likelihood of misidentification.
12 13 14
See State v. Cherry, 289 N.J. Super. 503 (App. Div. 1995). Id.
United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. at 233, 87 S.Ct. at 1935.
IDENTIFICATION:OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 4 of 5
15 16 17 18 19 20
other
witnesses, to photographs or newspaper accounts, or to any other
information or influence that may have affected the independence of
his/her identification.21
[CHARGE IN ALL CASES:] (7) Any other factor based on the evidence or lack of evidence in the case which you consider relevant to your determination of whether the out-of-court identification was reliable. [(8) Jury should be charged on any other relevant factor present in the case22] Id. Id. Id. Id., 87 S.Ct. at 1935-1936. See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting State v. Ramirez, 817 P.2d 774, 781 (Utah 1991)). See N.J. Attorney Generals Guidelines, supra, Guideline I.B. (requiring administrator to instruct witness that
perpetrator may not be present); State v. Ledbetter, 881 A.2d 290 (Ct. 2005) (requiring jury instruction to that effect).
21 22
See Herrera, 187 N.J. at 509 (quoting Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 781 n. 2 (citing State v. Long, 721 P.2d 483, 494 n. 8 (Utah 1986)).
The
list of factors enumerated in Biggers and Madison is not exhaustive.
See State v. White, 158 N.J. 230, (1999) (in declining to find plain
error in identification charge, court notes that instruction went beyond
model charge, noting the discrepancy ... between identifications made
by different witnesses). Additional relevant factors that should be
brought to jurys attention include the witnesss inability to make an
in-court identification if asked to do so while on the witness stand,
any failure on the part of the State to record a line-up or preserve a
photo array, as bearing upon the probative value of the out-of-court
identification, see State v. Delgado, 188 N.J. 48, 63 (2006); State
v. Earle, 60 N.J. 550, 552 (1972); State v. Peterkin, 226 N.J. Super.
25, 46 (App. Div. 1988), and any discrepancies between identifications
made by different witnesses, State v. White, 158
IDENTIFICATION:OUT-OF-COURT IDENTIFICATION ONLY
Page 5 of 5 [IN THE APPROPRIATE CASE,23 CHARGE THE FOLLOWING FACTOR:] (9) The fact that an identifying witness is not of the same race as the perpetrator and/or defendant, and whether that fact might have had an impact on the accuracy of the witnesss original perception, and/or the accuracy of the subsequent identification. You should consider that in ordinary human experience, people may have greater difficulty in accurately identifying members of a different race.24 [CHARGE IN ALL CASES:] Unless the out-of-court identification resulted from the witnesss observations or perceptions of the perpetrator during the commission of the offense, rather than being the product of an impression gained at the out-of-court identification procedure, it should be afforded no weight. The ultimate issue of the trustworthiness of the identification is for you to decide. If, after considering all the evidence, you determine that the State has not proven beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was the person who committed this offense [these offenses], then you must find him/her not guilty. If, on the other hand, after considering all of the evidence, you are convinced beyond a reasonable doubt that (defendant) was correctly identified, you will then consider whether the State has proven each and every element of the offense[s] charged beyond a reasonable doubt.
N.J. 230, 248.
23
An instruction that cross-racial identification is a factor to be considered should be given only when ... identification is
a critical issue in the case, and an eyewitnesss cross-racial identification is not corroborated by other evidence giving it
independent reliability. State v. Cromedy, 158 N.J. at 132; see also State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007). 24
Cromedy
holds that in order for the jury to determine the reliability of a
cross-racial identification not corroborated by independent evidence,
the jury must be informed of the potential risks associated with such
identifications, that the jury must be instructed about the possible
significance of the cross-racial identification factor.... 158 N.J. at
132-33. In State v. Romero, 191 N.J. 59 (2007), the New Jersey Supreme
Court refused to rule that cross-ethnic charges were required in cases
involving an individuals identification of a person of another ethnic
background.
|
Consequences of a Criminal Guilty Plea 1. You will have to appear in open court and tell the judge what you did that makes you guilty of the particular offense(s) 2. Do you understand that if you plead guilty: a. You will have a criminal record3. If you are on Probation, you will have to submit to random drug and urine testing. If you violate Probation, you often go to jail. 4. In indictable matters, you will be required to provide a DNA sample, which could be used by law enforcement for the investigation of criminal activity, and pay for the cost of testing. 5. You must pay restitution if the court finds there is a victim who has suffered a loss and if the court finds that you are able or will be able in the future to pay restitution. 6. If you are a public office holder or employee, you can be required to forfeit your office or job by virtue of your plea of guilty. 7. If you are not a United States citizen or national, you may be deported by virtue of your plea of guilty. 8. You must wait 5-10 years to expunge a first offense. 2C:52-3 9. You could be put on Probation. 10. In Drug Cases, a mandatory DEDR penalty of $500-$1,000, and lose your drivers license for 6 months - 2 years. You must pay a Law Enforcement Officers Training and Equipment Fund penalty of $30. 11. You may be required to do Community Service. 12. You must pay a minimum Violent Crimes Compensation Board assessment of $50 ($100 minimum if you are convicted of a crime of violence) for each count to which you plead guilty. 13. You must pay a $75 Safe Neighborhood Services Fund assessment for each conviction. 14. If you are being sentenced to probation, you must pay a fee of up to $25 per month for the term of probation. 15. You lose the presumption against incarceration in future cases. 2C:44-1 16. You may lose your right to vote. The defense of a person charged with a criminal offense is not impossible. There are a number of viable defenses and arguments which can be pursued to achieve a successful result. Advocacy, commitment, and persistence are essential to defending a client accused of a criminal offense. Jail for Crimes and Disorderly Conduct: If someone pleads Guilty or is found Guilty of a criminal offense, the following is the statutory Prison/Jail terms. NJSA 2C: 43-8 (1) In the case of a crime of the first degree, for a specific term of years which shall be fixed by the court and shall be between 10 years and 20 years; (2) In the case of a crime of the second degree, for a specific term of years which shall be fixed by the court and shall be between five years and 10 years; (3) In the case of a crime of the third degree, for a specific term of years which shall be fixed by the court and shall be between three years and five years; (4) In the case of a crime of the fourth degree, for a specific term which shall be fixed by the court and shall not exceed 18 months. 2C:43-3 Fines have been increased recently! 2C:43-3. Fines and Restitutions. A person who has been convicted of an offense may be sentenced to pay a fine, to make restitution, or both, such fine not to exceed: a. (1) $200,000.00 when the conviction is of a crime of the first degree; (2) $150,000.00 when the conviction is of a crime of the second degree; b. (1) $15,000.00 when the conviction is of a crime of the third degree; (2) $10,000.00 when the conviction is of a crime of the fourth degree; c. $1,000.00, when the conviction is of a disorderly persons offense; d. $500.00, when the conviction is of a petty disorderly persons offense; If facing any criminal charge, retain an experienced attorney immediately to determine you rights and obligations to the court. Current criminal charge researched by Kenneth Vercammen, Esq. 732-572-0500 |
No comments:
Post a Comment